Volumes of research have been published on natural syntactic development, but most of it, especially that of the last thirty years, is irrelevant to what goes on in our schools. David Crystal, for example, summarizes a famous experiment by Carol Chomsky in which she examines children’s ability to distinguish the meanings of "hard to see" and "easy to see":
What Carol Chomsky did was to present a group of children, aged from five upwards, with a blindfolded doll, and ask them "Is this doll easy to see or hard to see?" If the children said "Easy to see" then it is argued that they have learned the distinction; but if they said "Hard to see," and amplified their comment (upon request) by for instance, "Because it’s got a blindfold on," then it is argued that they had not learned it. The Chomsky results showed that before the age of six this distinction was hardly ever learned, whereas after seven it was known to nearly all children in the sample. (49)The statement that such research is "pedagogically irrelevant" requires amplification. As Crystal quite correctly goes on to note: "it is possible to extract a general conclusion from this and similar experiments, namely, that there are matters of structural (and in this case also semantic) interpretation which it takes children many years to acquire." But once we accept the idea that syntactic development occurs over a period of years, perhaps even throughout an individual’s whole life, no further pedagogical conclusions can be drawn from such research: it suggests nothing about what we could or should teach. At age five, most children have trouble with this particular distinction; at age eight, most of them have mastered it. But they have figured it out for themselves -- no "instruction" was involved, no "pedagogy."
Hunt's "T-Unit"
The work of these researchers centered on the child’s syntactic development between the ages of five and eighteen. The sentences of adults are obviously different than the sentences of children, and researchers had been looking for some way to measure the differences. A simple measure of words per sentence does not work because third, fourth, and some fifth graders create long sentences by compounding main clauses with "and." Hunt wondered if a count per main clause (defined as including all subordinate clauses) would be a better measure. Perhaps because the grammarians cannot agree about their terms (See Chapter One.), Hunt used the term "T-unit" -- for "minimal terminable unit." (In doing so, of course, he simply added to the terminological chaos.) To denote what he was measuring, he used the term "maturity," but he was careful to define it:
In this study the word "maturity" is intended to designate nothing more than "the observed characteristics of writers in an older grade." It has nothing to do with whether older students write "better" in any general stylistic sense. [Grammatical p. 5]Hunt convincingly demonstrated the validity of the T-unit as the "basic" unit of measurement. I emphasize "basic" because Hunt himself notes, as we will see, that it is not the only gauge. All of the subsequent major research studies have used Hunt's "T-unit" as their fundamental yardstick.
The figure for "Adults" is from Hunt’s study of essays published in The Atlantic and Harper’s. Although there are numerous questions surrounding such statistical studies, the basic conclusion remains valid: if samples of writing are taken from students (of similar socio-economic status) at different grade levels, the average number of words per main clause will show an increase similar to that above. Likewise, a study of passages from a variety of published writers will often result in a count of approximately twenty words per main clause.
The Natural Development of Subordinate Clauses
Hunt, O'Donnell, and Loban all concluded that subordinate clauses "blossom" (to use Hunt's term) between seventh and eighth grades. The following table is based on O'Hare's summary (p. 22) of Hunt's and O'Donnell's studies.
Sub Clauses
Grade / 100 Main Clauses Increase
3 18
4 29 + 11
5 27 - 2
7 30 + 3
8 42 + 12
12 68 + 26 (over four years)
Superior Adults 74 + 6
We need to be cautious with these results. The numbers for fourth, eighth,
twelfth, and "Superior Adults" come from Hunt's study; the others, from
O'Donnell's. Differences in the way constructions were defined and counted
may make the results not totally comparable. The table, however, does suggest
a major "blossoming" of subordinate clauses between seventh and eighth
grades.
Perhaps the most interesting study
here, however, is Loban's. As its title suggests (Language Development:
Kindergarten through Grade Twelve), Loban's study is the most comprehensive.
In addition to its time span, Loban, like O'Donnell, studied both oral
and written language. But he also divided the students whose sentences
he studied into three groups, high, low, and random. The following graph
(from page 39) clearly reflects the sharp increase in subordinate clauses
between seventh and eighth grade for the low and random groups. Note that
the "high" group shows a much slower increase during this grade level,
and that, between eighth and ninth grades, all three groups show a decrease,
the decrease being most significant for the high group. (I'll have more
to say about this later.)
Because even teachers of kindergarten
may see what appear to be numerous subordinate clauses in the writing of
their students, it is important to note that we are looking at a complicated
question. O'Donnell, for example, wrote that "One of the most enigmatic
features in the whole array of data collected in this study is the showing
that kindergarten children used relative clauses more frequently than did
children at any other stage, in either speech or writing." (60) It is "enigmatic,"
of course, because what appears to be a sign of increasing maturity decreases
instead of increasing with age. It is highly possible that the relative
clauses in the writing of kindergarten children are formulaic. (See below.)
All of the researchers attempted to break "subordinate clauses" into subcategories,
but such distinctions become very tenuous unless the transcripts of the
original data are available for review.
Until we get that research, the
only conclusive studies we have are those of Hunt, O'Donnell, and Loban,
all of which clearly show that subordinate clauses naturally blossom
in seventh and eighth grades. Attempts to get entire classes of students
to use more subordinate clauses in their writing before seventh grade may,
as Hake and Williams suggest, be counterproductive. (See Chapter Two.)
O'Donnell and the Concept of "Formulas"
In his section on "Conclusions and Implications," O'Donnell parenthetically alludes to "formulas." Formulas create a serious problem, both for anyone doing statistical research on natural syntactic development, and for some teachers. In the first paragraph of the following, O'Donnell comes as close as he ever does to suggesting a sequence of natural development beyond subordinate clauses. In the second, he mentions "formulas."
On the other hand, there was a group of items that appeared more than sporadically in kindergarten speech but were used from about three to ten times oftener by seventh graders. At various levels, there were significant increments in their use. These would appear good candidates for identification as generally later acquisitions. They were noun modification by a participle or participial phrase, the gerund phrase, the adverbial infinitive, the sentence adverbial, the coordinated predicate, and the transformation-produced nominal functioning as object of a preposition.The first paragraph supports the basic conclusions mentioned previously, and it extends them into areas that will not be covered in this book. Coordinated predication (She read a book and then took a nap), for example, would be an interesting area for research. Some researchers have counted this as two separate main clauses, and others have reported conflicting results about when and how often they appear. It should be obvious, however, that such coordination may have a significant affect on main-clause length.
Theoretically, it seems reasonable to suppose that these constructions (unless acquired as formulas) would be mastered relatively late. Transformational grammar derives them all by application of deletion rules, and some of them indirectly from their sources by way of strings that could more directly yield subordinate clauses. Thus, The man wearing a coat . . . may be more difficult than The man who was wearing a coat . . ., and A bird in the tree . . . more difficult than A bird that was in the tree . . . . Noting that noun clauses did not vary much in frequency after the first grade, while participial modifiers of nouns were used by seventh graders three times as often in speech and nearly eight times as often in writing as they were used by kindergarten children, we may contend that such clauses (The dove saw that the ant was drowning ) are easier to manage and earlier added to the child's repertory than is the reduction of them to a single participial modifier (The dove saw the ant drowning). (92)
The child may operate with subordinate clauses, with words like "because," "if," "when," and "but," long before he really grasps causal, conditional, or temporal relationships. He masters syntax of speech before syntax of thought. Piaget’s studies proved that grammar develops before logic and that the child learns relatively late the mental operations corresponding to the verbal forms he has been using for a long time. (46)It is very possible, therefore, that the subordinate clauses, appositives, participles, etc. in the writing of most pre-seventh graders are formulaic.
Throughout this chapter I
have suggested that these researchers have shown that some grammatical
constructions develop later than others. The general outline of that development
appears to be 1) the development of basic sentences through sixth grade,
2) the blooming of subordinate clauses in seventh and eighth grades, and
3) the late blooming of constructions such as the gerundive and appositive.
Hunt was the researcher who explored and expressed this sequence the most
clearly and forcefully.
It is tempting to say that
Hunt even had a theory about why syntactic development occurs both in this
sequence and as slowly as it does. From my perspective,
99% of the KISS theory of natural syntactic development (Part Two of this
book) is clearly implied in Hunt's work. If Hunt didn't develop the theoretical
aspects of his work, it was probably because he had started with the objective
of finding a way of measuring syntactic growth, and, once he found
it, he had to explain it. This is clear in his 1966 article, "Recent Measures
in Syntactic Development." This eight-page article is an excellent introduction
to Hunt's work. The basic objective of the article is to explain the "T-unit"
and "subordinate clause index" as fundamental measures of increasing maturity.
If we look at the article more closely, however, the concepts that Hunt
explains, and the examples that he uses, contain almost everything one
needs for a theory. Hunt is, we should note, very tentative in his presentation:
The science of measuring syntactic maturity is barely emerging from the stages of alchemy. It scarcely deserves to be called a science at all. But we do know a few things.Hunt's focus is on "measuring," but he is clearly cautioning against rushing natural syntactic development.
For the last thirty years we have known at least three things about the development of language structure. First, as children mature they tend to produce more words on any given subject. They have more to say. Second, as children mature, the sentences they use tend to be longer. Third, as children mature a larger proportion of their clauses are subordinate clauses.
In the last two years it has been possible to add a few more measures, and I will come to them later. But first let me turn back to the statement about subordinate clauses and try to make clear its significance for the teaching program. It would be worse than useless for a fourth-grade teacher to say to her students, "Now if you will go back to your last paper and add more subordinate clauses to the main clauses, you will be writing like Miss Hill's wonderful sixth graders or Miss Summit's wonderful eighth graders instead of my own miserable fourth graders." Such an approach would be worse than useless. (732, my emphasis)
But of course subordinating clauses is not all there is to syntactic development. In every pair of examples I have given so far, it would have been possible to reduce one of the clauses still further so that it is no longer a clause at all, but merely a word or phrase consolidated inside the other clause. In this fashion two clauses will become one clause. The one clause will now be one word or one phrase longer than it was before, but it will be shorter than the two clauses were together. By throwing away some of one clause we will gain in succinctness. The final expression will be tighter, less diffuse, more mature. (734)Hunt here clearly proposes a basic outline of natural syntactic development. Children develop mastery of the basic main clause, then expand it by embedding one clause into another (subordination), and then learn to tighten their sentences by reducing subordinate clauses to less-than-clause constructions.
We caught two bass, hauling them in briskly as though they were mackerel, pulling them over the side of the boat in a businesslike manner without any landing net, and stunning them with a blow on the back of the head.Hunt reports that of the 300 people who rewrote "Aluminum," "not one of them produced this construction" (my emphasis). Perhaps because no one used it for him, Hunt here switches to a discussion of the "Chicken" passage. He states:
Out of 10 fourth graders who rewrote "The Chicken," not even one produced it. By 10 eighth graders who rewrote it, it was produced once:The gerundive is a late-blooming construction -- currently, perhaps as late as college. We also need to keep in mind Loban's clear demonstration that students do not progress through the natural sequence of syntactic development simultaneously. The fact that one eighth grader produced a gerundive does not mean that all eighth graders should be forced to study, or, worse yet, try to use, gerundives.
She slept all the time, laying no eggs.
By 10 twelfth graders this construction was produced twice. Here are both examples.
The chicken cackled, waking the man.
Blaming the chicken, he killed her and ate her for breakfast.
But the university students produced 14 examples. In fact, 9 out of 10 university students studied produced at least one example, whereas only 1 out of 10 twelfth graders had done so. In the little time between high school and the university, this construction suddenly burst into bloom." (100, my emphasis)
Near the end of the article,
Hunt does suggest the application of the "blooming" sequence to teaching.
"The kind of information given previously as to which structures bloom
early and which bloom late would be preliminary to actual measures of teachability
at a given level" (102). This sentence clearly implies that certain
constructions should be taught at later grade levels, but it was never
developed and has been ignored. Thus we have sentence combining exercises
for second graders in which they are required to use appositives, and even,
in some cases, gerundives. If, instead of trying to rush instruction into
earlier grade levels, we could follow the natural syntactic development
suggested by the research, wouldn't we have a much more effective and theoretically
justified approach to teaching grammar?
Hunt, O'Donnell, and Loban were
more interested in research than in theory. As a group, they convincingly
demonstrated that the ability to generate subordinate clauses (as opposed
to using formulas) blossoms between seventh and ninth grades. Appositives
and gerundives bloom after students have mastered subordinate clauses,
probably because they are generally reductions of such clauses. Hunt was,
as I suggested at the beginning of this section, very close to the KISS
theory of natural syntactic development. In fact, he was extremely close.
In Part Two of this book, we will see just how close.
[The longer, web version of this chapter is in the section on "Essays
on Grammar." In addition to discussing some of the points discussed here
(such as formulas and late-blooming constructions) in more detail, it also
explores why the work of Hunt, O'Donnell, and Loban was not pursued, and
what problems will be faced by further research.]