When most people think of grammar,
they think of avoiding errors. But grammar includes much more than that,
and is more important than that. Generally speaking, grammar can be divided
into two areas -- usage and syntax (sentence structure). Usage includes
"rules of etiquette" -- "Don't write ain't." "Don't use a double
comparative (such as more better)." Most of these rules are in the
negative imperative ("Don't"), and they make people, especially students,
worry about and hate grammar. Syntax, on the other hand, concerns the basic
building blocks of sentences -- prepositional phrases, subjects and verbs,
clauses. This distinction, unfortunately, is not understood even by many
of the people who teach English grammar in our schools. But the distinction
is vital.
The KISS approach concerns syntax,
and if you study it, you will be surprised at how few building blocks are
needed to understand how any sentence in English is put together -- how
it conveys (or fails to convey) intended meaning. Etiquette (usage) may
be important, but meaning (syntax) is more so. Some students naturally
develop an excellent command of syntax, especially if they do a lot of
reading. But many students do not. They write short, weak sentences that
are simply not capable of expressing the complex thoughts required both
in college and in the workplace. And not only do they write poorly, but
their reading skills are very weak as well. Such students suffer in college
and in the workplace, and they will continue to do so because our schools
never properly address the teaching of syntax.
The closest our schools have
ever come to doing a decent job of teaching syntax was when they taught
sentence diagramming. Sentence diagrams show how the various words in a
sentence meaningfully relate to each other. (And, as I will attempt to
show below, it is a matter of the syntax affecting the meaning.) In our
schools, however, diagramming generally failed for at least five reasons.
First, the rules for which lines go where on the diagram are cumbersome.
Second, diagramming a complicated sentence takes a lot of time, effort,
and paper. Third, the sentences used in the classroom were almost all too
simplistic. Fourth, teachers often did not understand how to diagram, and
therefore only used "safe" sentences for which they had answer keys. Fifth,
students were rarely told why they were learning to diagram.
Although some schools continue
to teach diagramming, most have abandoned it. Here is not the place to
examine all of the things that have been tried in its place, but the most
common is to teach students to identify specific constructions. For example,
students will study and then do an exercise on subordinate clauses. The
exercise usually contains from ten to twenty carefully selected (and thus
overly simplistic) sentences. For years, teachers have been mystified by
the fact that most students can do these exercises, but the exercises have
no effect on the students' ability to read or write. Perhaps the mystery
can be explained by an analogy. Suppose I were to teach you about computers.
First, I'll teach you about disk drives. I'll show you, and teach you to
identify, several types of drives. I'll even have you look inside computers
and point to the drives (which is comparable to having students identify
clauses in a sentence). Then we'll work on mother boards; then video cards,
then modems, then audio cards. When we are finished, I'll give you all
the parts you need -- plus some extras -- and ask you to put together some
computers. Do you think you could do it?
If you think you could, you
might want to try it. I have been working with computers for almost two
decades, but I still hate to have to go inside one. And I'll probably never
try to put one together from scratch. I know all the parts, but the connections
among them get very confusing. Which chip goes in which socket? Which cable
plugs into what? It's the connections that cause the problem. And it's
the connections that cause many students problems with sentence structure.
Learning to identify parts is a first step, but unless one has had some
practice with the connections -- in real computers, or in real sentences
-- the ability to identify the parts is practically useless.
The KISS approach teaches students
to identify the parts and the connections in real sentences. The students'
objective is to be able to explain how every word in every sentence is
syntactically connected to a main subject / verb / complement pattern.
To my knowledge, no approach to teaching grammar has ever done this before.
The sentences in these editions of the KISS Grammar Game, for example,
were written by a Professor of Biology and by a nursing student. Although
they were aware that their writing would be used in a grammar game, neither
writer was aware of how the game works, or how they might want to edit
their writing for the game. They simply wrote as they normally would. The
editions therefore present real sentences in real contexts, and the game
deals with each and every sentence, in sequence.
Because the game deals with
real sentences in context, students can see themselves making progress
toward their objective of understanding all of the connections in any sentence.
They can literally measure their progress. The following chart is an example:
|
||
Level | Biology Edition | Nursing Edition |
1. Prepositional Phrases |
|
|
2. + Adjectivves, Adverbs, & CConj. |
|
|
3. + words in S/V/C slots |
|
|
4. + Clauses |
|
|
5. + Verbals (Gerund, gerundive, infinitive) |
|
|
6. + 8 other constructions |
|
|