Level Two Instructional Materials
An Introduction to Teaching Subjects, Finite Verbs, and Complements The first thing we need to remember here is
that pre-school children are all masters of subjects, finite
verbs, and complements. With rare exceptions, everyone uses them correctly
almost all the time. But because subjects, finite verbs, and their complements
are the core of English sentence structure, students need to be able to
identify these parts of sentences in order to understand how sentences
work.
Many teachers have noted that their students
have little trouble with the exercises in their grammar books, but that
when the students try to analyze their own writing, they are very confused
and make lots of errors. This happens because learning to identify
finite verbs can be extremely simple or very difficult, depending on the
objectives and exercises. Most textbooks give students simple definitions
and then very simple sentences for exercises. If the sentences in the exercises
contain only one verb, which is often the case, students will normally
learn to identify the verb relatively quickly. But if the sentences contain
many verbs, as the sentences that students themselves write often do, then
students will often be very confused.
A Note on the Difficulty of KISS Level Two The instructional material for KISS Level Two is almost certainly the most difficult of all the KISS levels for students to master. Once we go beyond the typical simplified instructional material and exercises, we are asking students to identify the subjects, finite verbs, and complements in any written text. In KISS Level One, we can give students a very small list of words that function as prepositions and ask the students to start by finding those words in a text. No such limited list is available for the thousands of verbs in the language. Instead, we can start with some simple examples, but it will take students a fair amount of time and practice -- and a good number of mistakes -- to use their unconscious command of S/V/C patterns to learn to identify and label such patterns. Compared to this, KISS Level Three is easy. At that level, students simply need to look at the S/V/C patterns that they have identified and use a few rules to determine the boundaries and types of clauses. And once clauses have been mastered, KISS Levels Four and Five supply some relatively simple rules and definitions for explaining the few remaining words in any sentence that student may read or write. Mastering KISS Level Two will require time, practice, and patience. Identifying Subjects and Finite Verbs It, was, I believe, Paul Roberts, my
favorite grammarian, who noted that the most difficult part of teaching
grammar is in helping students to be able to identify finite
verbs. The instructional material provided
here is probably too much for primary school students. Parents and
teachers can adapt it to suit their needs, and even very young students
can be exposed to some of it. Personally, however, I do not believe that
it is very helpful. I have included it because it is what is found in many
textbooks. There is a better way to achieve the objective, and that is
simply to give students numerous examples, followed by exercises based
on relatively simple sentences.
Helping Verbs, Auxiliary Verbs, ModalsGrammar textbooks use these three terms to distinguish basically the same group of words that may function as parts of verb phrases. "Auxiliary" derives from the Latin for "to help," so these are simply two terms for the same thing. "Modal" relates to "modulate." Roberts states that "In general the modal auxiliaries are used to express not statements of fact but actions or events that exist only as conceptions of the mind -- possibilities, potentialities, necessities, wishes, whatever may or may not eventuate in the future." (Understanding Grammar, 171) In essence, these are questions of usage, and not sentence structure. Students, however, often do no see these words as parts of verb phrases, and thus this list may be helpful in that respect. One of the primary reasons for the failures of most grammar textbooks is that they explain the basic sentence patterns in terms of "subjects" and "predicates." "Predicate" is an important term in philosophical discussions of what and how sentences "mean," but it results in major problems for students once they move beyond very simple sentences. Thus, in In addition, the "subject" and "predicate" distinction masks the importance of complements and makes it more difficult for most students to identify subjects in multi-S/V/C pattern sentences. Suppose, for example, that the example above included a subordinate clause: In the morning, we visited the town that was destroyed in the war. Most textbooks do not even discuss "predicates" in terms of sentences
such as this, but their underlying logic would suggest that "we" is still
the subject, and that the "predicate" is "visited the town that was destroyed
in the war." That is fine for the first S/V/C pattern in this sentence,
but the students also need to be able to find the subject of "was destroyed."
If they follow the rule that most textbooks give them, the students will
ask "Who or what was destroyed?" The answer to that is "town," but "town"
is not, even in those grammar books, the subject. And, since the grammar
books do not teach students to recognize S/V/C patterns, the students have
no way of even understanding why "town" is not the right answer.
The Types of ComplementsTextbook definitions of complements are often confusing and inaccurate. For example, the fifteenth edition (2004) of the Harbrace Handbook states that "A direct object usually receives the action expressed by the verb. It appears after the verb." (15) In the first place, what is meant by "usually" and by "receives the action"? What "action" is expressed by "keeps" in "He keeps his tools in the garage"? Is this one of the exceptions expressed in "usually"? How is a student supposed to know? And if a direct object appears after the verb, then what is "Bill" in "Bill she adores"? The Harbrace example is typical, but it illustrates only part of the basic problem of the textbooks. The textbooks are geared toward naming and defining various grammatical constructions. I am unaware of any textbook that even claims to try to enable students to identify (and thus intelligently discuss) these constructions in real sentences from texts that the students read and write. And over the years various names have been used to identify the same, or sometimes very different constructions. Most textbooks, for example, try to define and "teach" a distinction among "transitive," "intransitive," and "linking" verbs. Consider, however, the following comments by Paul Roberts in Understanding Grammar: Transitive verbs are often defined as verbs that require objects to complete their meaning and intransitive verbs as verbs that do not. This is misleading, for it implies that transitivity resides in the form of the verb, or in the form and its associated meaning. Grammarians who adopt this view are led to say that eat in "Let's eat" is transitive, even though it has no object, because one cannot eat without eating something. It would seem to follow that sang in "She sang beautifully" is transitive, because one cannot sing without singing something, viz., a song. Yet probably all grammarians would call sang intransitive in this context. (117-118)The preceding should give you some idea of what most grammarians mean by "transitive" and "intransitive," but more importantly, it clearly indicates that the grammarians themselves disagree. And if the grammarians cannot agree, what are students supposed to do? In essence, KISS basically ignores "transitive," "intransitive," and "linking" as analytical concepts. The fundamental problem in the teaching of grammar is that the terminology is too complex and simply overwhelming. I will never forget the post on the NCTE-talk list in which a teacher tried to argue that we should teach grammar and those "transient" and "intransient" verbs. If teachers cannot keep the terms straight, how can we expect students to do so? As a matter of vocabulary, students should probably learn the meaning of "transitive," "intransitive," and "linking." Most dictionaries, for example, indicate transitive (vt) and intransitive (vi) verbs. The terms are also used in general discussions of language. There is, if I remember correctly, even a book titled Write Is a Transitive Verb. Within the KISS framework this is easily taught -- the verbs in S/V patterns with no complement are intransitive; the verbs in S/V/PN or S/V/PA patterns are "linking"; and the verbs in S/V/(IO)DO patterns are transitive. The disagreement in the naming of types of verbs is also reflected in the naming of complements. Consider the following paragraph, also from Roberts' UG: Linking verbs are often called copulas or copulative verbs; copula is simply a Latin version of linking. There is no general agreement about what to call the substantive or modifier which the linking verb links to the subject. This book joins those that call it the subjective complement. It is a "complement" in that it completes the meaning of the clause, and it is "subjective" because it renames or describes the subject. The virtue of this term is that it suggests the prime function of the construction -- the identification of the subject with some other word. Other terms used are predicate nominative, predicate noun, predicate pronoun, predicate adjective. The subjective complement may be a noun, a pronoun, an adjective, a phrase, or a clause. (116)Here again the passage reflects disagreement among grammarians and a wide range of terms to denote the same (or different) things. One pedagogical problem with "subjective complement" is that, as Roberts defines it, "completes the meaning of the clause" is vague. In some grammars, the terms "completer" is used in place of "complements," but in these grammars adverbial phrases are included in the "completers." Thus in "They were on their way," "on their way" is considered a "completer." For students, in other words, "completes" introduces another area of potential confusion. A more important problem with "subjective complement" is that it combines predicate nouns and predicate adjectives (the terms that KISS uses) under one name and thus blurs an important distinction. To understand the problem, we need to step back to the broader question of why we should want students to learn about the types of complements, in the first place. The answer to that question is really a matter of composition and logic. Some students have serious problems with the S/V/PN pattern. They use a verb that means "equals," but they often do no mean that the subject of the sentence is equal to the complement. Or maybe they do. As students' sentences naturally become longer and more complex, more and more words come between the subject and the complement. And often the pattern slips. Did the student who wrote "My girlfriend . . . is . . . a beautiful body" really mean that? If he did, then his girlfriend should have dumped him. More likely, however, is that the pattern slipped. The following sentence, written by a college Freshman, is one of my favorite examples: The taste of a sizzling foot-long hotdog coated with tangy sauerkraut with mounds of pickle relish is a typical snack when accompanied by a tall, chilled paper cup of Coke.The basic pattern of this sentence is "... taste . . . is . . . snack . . ." Robert's "subjective complement" might help students see the problem here, but the KISS systematic approach is probably clearer and simpler for the students. "Snack" is not an adjective (nor does it describe "taste"), so is cannot be a predicate adjective. Next, although the verb means "equals," the "snack" is not equal to the "taste," is is equal to the "hotdog." There are other reasons for distinguishing predicate adjectives from predicate nouns, but perhaps the most important one is that formal definitions normally begin with an S/V/PN, and not an S/V/PA pattern. No matter what field a student goes into, definitions will be important. And most instructors in other fields expect definitions to begin with an S/V/PN pattern (even if they cannot explain the grammatical terms). "A saw is a tool." is the beginning of a formal definition. "A saw is sharp" is not. Students who have been taught to distinguish predicate nouns from predicate adjectives will find it much easier to understand formal definitions. As a final word on the types of complements, let me note that I literally tell my college students that I do not care, and will not take points off, if they label indirect objects as direct objects. That statement will strike horror into the hearts of many grammarians, but the grammarians have been completely unable to teach students much about grammar. If they could, then students would not be entering college totally unable to identify verbs. The grammarians would want me to spend time, as they do, on very simple sentences in order to teach students this distinction. Since no student ever has any problems using indirect and direct objects, at the college level I simply do not have time for teaching the distinction. Predicate Adjective or Part of the Verb? Faced with a sentence such as "The boat was
tied to the pier," many of my students initially mark "tied" as a predicate
adjective. Most grammar textbooks would rightfully consider "was tied"
to be a passive verb, but there is also excellent logic behind the students'
response. Passive verbs slide into S/V/PA patterns, or, in other words,
the two grammatical concepts form a continuum. In "The eggs were scrambled
to Bill's satisfaction," the scrambling," and who scrambled them, are not
matters of concern. "Scrambled" describes the eggs and really functions
as a predicate adjective.
Teaching students to distinguish finite verbs from verbals. Finite verbs are not easy to define. Perhaps
that is why even many experienced English teachers do not know what they
are. (Ask some.) This does not, by the way, suggest that teachers are stupid.
The problem is that the professors who teach the teachers, and the people
who write the textbooks, are more interested in teaching the names of constructions.
They do not even attempt to help K-12 teachers learn how to analyze the
sentences that students read and write. Thus, instead of using the term
"verbals," the professors (and the textbooks) focus on the three types
of verbals -- "gerunds," "participles" (KISS "gerundives"), and "infinitives."
This "Divide and Confuse" strategy keeps power and money in the hands of
the professors and textbook publishers, but it does not help the teachers
and students.
An Introduction to Finite Verbs and VerbalsThe "Introduction" gives students a brief general explanation. The three "tests" provide explanations and examples of ways to make the distinctions. The "Sentence Test" usually covers the examples in the "Noun" and "To" tests, but the "Noun: and "To" tests are easier to remember and to apply. Thus the KISS exercises are also separated, with some devoted specifically to the Noun Test, some to the "To" Test, and some to the Sentence Test. This separation should help teachers help students master one test at a time. It would be wonderful if these three tests covered every possible case, but life is not that simple. A conservative, educated guess would be that they will enable students to identify 95 + % of the verbals in what they read and write, but they do miss some things. Consider, for example, the sentence: Two Notes of Caution:
Varied Positions in S/V/C PatternsThe fifteenth edition (2004) of the Harbrace Handbook literally defines direct objects as coming after the verb (p. 15), but that is not always true. It is true that they usually appear after the verb, so some students are confused when then appear before it. If they use the KISS procedure for finding complements, students should not have any real trouble, but this short bit of instructional material may add to their confidence. It may also, of course, encourage them to occasionally vary the patterns of their own sentences. Understood "You"Some students can figure this out on their own, but the instructional material should make it clear for all students. Traditional grammars refer to these sentences as "Imperatives" or the "Imperative Mood." Some grammars simply use the term "Commands." Obviously, you can, if you wish, teach students these names, but remember that the primary problem in the teaching of grammar is an overabundance of terminology. CompoundsMost textbooks teach definitions and constructions rather than concepts. As a result, they often have separate explanations and exercises for identifying compound subjects, compound verbs, compound direct objects, compound clauses, etc. Rarely, however, do they suggest that any construction can be compounded. KISS includes numerous exercises on compounding because it is a fundamental aspect of natural syntactic development. As you will see, however, once students understand the basic concept, most of the KISS exercises are related to questions of style and logic. Multiple
Patterns in One Sentence
Pronouns as Subjects [See also: "Personal Pronouns."]Because pronouns basically function in any way that a noun can, KISS does not devote much attention to them. But as soon as students start to explore real texts, they will run into multiple S/V/C patterns in one sentence, and a fair number of these patterns will have pronouns in the subject slot. Given the sentence Almost all of the classification of pronouns in the instructional material is based on Paul Roberts' Understanding Grammar (NY: Harper & Row, 1954, pp. 46-89). Roberts devotes a fairly large amount of space to the classification and discussion of pronouns, including distinctions between "relative," "interrogative," and "demonstrative" pronouns. He also discusses the debate about whether "his" and "her" are pronouns or adjectives, and questions related to "case" -- for example, the "who" "whom" distinction. He begins, however, with "Problems of Definition" (53-56), and, as with most grammarians and their textbooks, much of the discussion is primarily of interest to grammarians. Every preschool child, for example, knows perfectly well how to form questions with "Who," "Which" and "What." Is there any value in requiring students to remember that, when they are used to form questions, these three words are called "Interrogative Pronouns"? Since research suggests that students are bewildered by the massive amount of typical grammatical terminology, it would seem that such a requirement may do more harm than good. The same is true, I would suggest, for "relative" and "demonstrative." Native speakers of English have no trouble using these pronouns correctly, and thus forcing the students to learn names for these categories appears to be not only an exercise in uselessness, but also counterproductive. The question of grammatical "case" is significantly different. Many students do have problems with the use of "who"/"whom," "I"/"me," etc., but I have never seen any evidence that an approach through formal definitions has any effect on how people use these words. The reason for that is that the problem itself results from two causes, neither of which is open to influence through formal definitions. One of these causes is simply the habit of informal usage. No theoretical explanation of "case" is going to stop some people from saying "Me and Bill went to the store." Most people who would currently write this hate grammar anyway, and they are certainly not about to seriously listen to a discussion of nominative, genitive (also called possessive), and objective case, when they have not even learned to identify pronouns in the first place. On the other hand, my students who have this problem do realize that putting themselves first ("Me and Bill") might make them appear to be selfish. Their peers also acknowledge this, thereby putting more pressure on the violators to put "Bill" first. These students also acknowledge that they should be able to drop "Bill" and still have an acceptable sentence. When they try that, they themselves note that "Me went to the store" is not acceptable English (unless one is Cookie Monster). The other primary cause for pronoun case errors is that the students have never been required to study the structure of real sentences. Put more simply, "he," "she," "they, "who," etc. are used for grammatical subjects; "him," "her," "them," "whom" etc. are used for "object" slots -- indirect and direct objects and objects of prepositions (and thus are called "objective case"). In other words, students who make mistakes usually do so because they cannot tell whether the word in question functions as a subject or as an object. The KISS Approach, of course, is directly aimed at enabling students to do just that, and as students regularly underline "who" and as subject and label "whom" as an indirect or direct object, or as the object of a preposition, they will themselves see the problem when a "whom" shows up in a subject slot. Indeed at that point students might actually be interested in the formal study of grammatical case. Current instruction, however, is futile and frustrating in that it simply teaches the categories and then drops them. Note that the instructional material is not totally accurate in regard to prepositional phrases. Subjects can be in prepositional phrases -- if the verbs that go with them are in the phrase also -- in other words, if a clause is the object of the preposition: They were
talking {to [whoever Some grammarians have pointed to this problem as a reason for saying that the KISS Approach will not work. But the problem is not at all serious. As students analyze randomly selected sentences and short texts, approximately one prepositional phrase in every two hundred will have a clause as its object. And in the KISS Approach, students should be told that they are expected to make mistakes. Besides, a 99.5% accuracy rate is an "A+" in any school classroom. And when they get to clauses in KISS Level Three, students will have little trouble with clauses that function as objects of prepositions. Ellipsis [Instructional Material] Because they focussed on individual constructions
rather than on the analysis of sentences, traditional grammars paid little
attention to ellipsis. Out of sight; out of mind. The newer, transformational
grammars demonstrated the importance of ellipsis, which they refer to as
"reduction." In the 1970's, researchers on the natural growth of of syntactic
complexity used the transformational concepts to suggest that reduction
is a major instrument for increasing sentence complexity. As a simple example,
the young child's "The house is on the corner. It is big." becomes "The
big house is on the corner." In effect, the "It is" in the second sentence
is reduced (ellipsed), and the important information ("big") is embedded
in the first sentence.
A "palimpsest" is a parchment or clay tablet which has been used more than once, the earlier writing having been erased. KISS defines a syntactic palimpsest pattern as one S/V/C pattern "written" over another. Such patterns are infrequent and usually idiomatic, but the concept does help explain the meaning of some sentences. An interesting example appears in the following sentence from Andrew Lang's version of "Thumbelina": There the mouse lived warm and snug, with a store-room full of corn, a splendid kitchen and dining-room.Since most people would consider "warm" and "snug" to be adjectives, and not adverbs, we can explain "the mouse lived warm and snug" as the pattern "the mouse lived" overlaid on "the mouse was warm and snug." For people who want to avoid the "Expletive" concept, the palimpsest pattern provides an explanation for the relatively rare cases in which the expletive is followed by a verb other than "to be" ("is," "are," "was," "were," etc.) Consider, for example, the opening sentence of "Little Red Riding Hood": Once upon a time there lived in a certain village a little country girl ....One way of explaining this is to consider "there" as the subject and "girl" as a delayed subject. Another, however, is to consider this as a palimpsest pattern with the pattern "There lived ...." as under (or over) the pattern "There was ... girl." From the perspective of understanding natural syntactic development, palimpsests may be very important. The general concepts of such development suggest that clauses may be compounded, and then reduced, embedded, and subordinated. Thus, for example,
Expletives (Optional Concept) The basic KISS perspective on expletives is explained in the KISS Differences. This concept could probably be totally deleted from pedagogical grammars. In a sentence such as I accept the "expletive" as an explanation from the rare student who has been taught and remembered it. Most of my college students have been taught grammar so uselessly and poorly that they do not remember the meaning of any grammatical terminology. Indeed, the most pedagogical grammars are so poorly thought through that they end up being useless. Thus most such grammars discuss "it" and "there" as expletives, but I have yet to see one that includes "here." But if "there" is an expletive in "There's a nice man," should we not also consider "here" to be one in "Here's a nice man."? Note that another alternative explanation for both "there' and "here" would be to consider them to be adverbs. Personal Pronouns [See also: "Pronouns as Subjects."] Fourth graders can certainly be introduced
to personal pronouns and the distinctions of first, second, and third person.
Practically speaking, however, students should probably do one or two exercises
on personal pronouns every year, simply so that they do not forget. In
reality, this is more a question of vocabulary than of sentence structure
-- students really do need to know what is meant by first, second, and
third person.
I went to Washington and visited the Museum of Natural History.
The first sentence establishes the writer's credentials for discussing
the Museum. In most contexts, there is absolutely nothing wrong with it,
and there is much to be said in its favor. The second sentence, on the
other hand, demonstrates that the writer does not know what an essay or
paper is -- any essay or paper is, after all, a statement of what the writer
thinks, believes, knows, etc. Good writers do occasionally use such phrases,
but they do so to indicate that they are presenting what they recognize
to be a relatively weak point in their argument. The "I think...,"
"I believe...," "in my opinion," etc. is like a flag that says to the reader,
"O.K. I'm not stupid. I know that this point is weaker than my other arguments.
But it is relevant and supports my position."
"No grammatical problem is more difficult than that of
tense."
Subjunctive
Mood
-- Paul Roberts, Understanding Grammar, 131. Some Comments on "Objective Complements" The KISS view of "objective complements" is explained in "The KISS Differences." Here I simply wish to suggest your options in handling this concept. In essence, some verbs appear to take two complements: The sound made me afraid. KISS, however, is intentionally minimalist in terminology -- it proposes the fewest terms possible for teaching students how to explain any word in any sentence. Because, in more complex cases, "objective" and "subjective" complements are defined differently by different grammarians, KISS simply eliminates them, but the KISS explanation of the phenomenon is based on ellipsed infinitives, a Level Four construction. If you are having students analyze randomly selected sentences, what should you do when such double complements show up? The KISS response is 1) for purposes of grading, ignore whatever the students do with them, 2) in the process of reviewing homework, simply tell students that the construction is an "advanced" one that they will study later, and 3) if the students appear interested, explain the concept but do not expect the students to master it, i.e., do not test them on it. This advice should work whether you decide to use either the KISS explanation or objective complements. The most important point here is that students need to master the fundamental concept first (in this case basic complements), and only after they have mastered it should we begin to expect them to master the more complex variations of it. This border presents Marc Chagall's (1887-1985) Russian I and the Village (1911 Museum of Modern Art, New York, NY) from Jim's Fine Art Collection http://www2.iinet.com/art/index.html [for educational use only] Click here for the directory of my backgrounds based on art. |