The KISS Grammar Approach
to Improving Writing
and to Grammatical Errors
(It's the same approach.) |
 |
Grant Wood's
American Gothic
(1891-1942) 1930,
Art Institute of Chicago |
Introduction
Too many teachers waste way too
much time teaching grammar in order to "help students avoid errors." It
simply does not work. Unfortunately, many teachers, who realize
that it does not work, continue to do so because they don't know what else
to do. (Now, of course, they will have the KISS Approach.) Those teachers
who think that it does work have never been able to prove so. If they had
been able to prove it, NCTE would not have passed a resolution
against the teaching of grammar. Indeed these teachers may be doing
more harm than good. They may, for example, focus on comma-splices and
then note fewer such splices appearing in students' writing. That effect,
however, is more likely the result of the well-known phenomenon of the
students writing shorter, safer sentences. The best way to deal with errors
is not to deal with them formally at all. As this essay suggests, if we
teach
grammar as a way to improve writing, the errors will probably disappear
on their own.
Currently, of course, most classroom
teachers cannot effectively teach all five KISS levels. Will KISS help
students improve their writing and avoid errors if the students can only
work with KISS Level One? The answer to that question is "definitely."
After some general remarks, this essay suggests how each of the five KISS
levels relates to these two questions. We need to keep in mind here that
KISS is a structured sequence of instruction that can be started at any
grade level. All students should start at KISS Level 1.1 and work their
way through the levels.
KISS Grammar as Instruction in
Writing
Before
KISS was as developed with exercises as it now is, some people had the
impression that I hate sentence-combining exercises. They were right --
and wrong. As an undergraduate, I signed up for a psychology course. I
thought I was going to be studying Freud. Instead I studied Skinner. I
spent three hours a week (for fifteen weeks) training a rat. It was one
of the most influential courses that I ever took. Typical books with sentence-combining
exercises remind me of that course. But students are not rats. Typical
books give students short sentences to combine into longer ones, but they
do not expect students to be able to identify the constructions they are
using to make the combinations. Nor do they give students a psycholinguistic
model of how our brains process language. Like the rats, students are not
expected to understand what they are doing. A short book could be written
about the problems caused by such instruction, but here I'll simply say
that KISS respects students' intelligence.
KISS includes
sentence combining and de-combining in the majority of the sections that
introduce new constructions. Even in KISS Level 1.3 (Adding Adjectives
and Adverbs) an exercise asks students to combine two simple sentences
(They live in a house. The house is big.) into one (They live in a big
house.) This may seem like an extremely simplistic exercise for older students,
but even for them it addresses a common complaint of college professors
(including many who teach subjects far removed from English and grammar)
-- for many college students, once a sentence is written, it is as solid
as cement. Thus, although the exercise is simple, it introduces the fundamental
idea that written sentences can be improved.
The overview
below explains how writing exercises are integrated into the learning to
identify various constructions. Remember, however, that each KISS Level
is followed by a "Practice/Application" book. These books include more
exercises from what is now called KISS Level Six. Most of the Level Six
sections apply the constructions that students have learned to identify
to questions of writing.
6.1 Studies in Punctuation
6.2 Style -- Focus, Logic, and
Texture
6.3 Style -- "Free" Sentence
Combining Exercises
6.4 Studies in the Syntax of Little
Words
6.5 Statistical Stylistics
6.6 Syntax and Writing
6.7 Additional Passages for Analysis
For more on what each of these sections
is intended to do, see the "booklet" that explains them.
The KISS Approach to Grammatical
Errors
Part of the problem with trying to
teach grammar in order to avoid errors is that almost no distinction has
been made among the three types of errors -- usage, syntax, and pronoun
reference. In addition, many teachers have never been taught that syntactic
errors should probably be welcomed instead of being squashed. (Many
errors are actually signs of growth -- or signs of poor instruction.) Usage
can be considered the clothing of language -- it may or may not be appropriate
for the occasion; syntax, on the other hand, is language's
skeleton -- without it, language is meaningless.
When I say that errors should
not be dealt with "formally," I mean that specific errors should, with
one exception, NEVER be the focus of class discussion. There is no reason
to do so, and there is a strong possibility that giving students examples
of errors, orally or in writing, not only reinforces the error among those
who make it, but also spreads it to those who don't. The remedies for the
two types of errors (usage and syntax) differ, and our exception concerns
errors of usage.
Errors in Usage
"Usage" involves the "Don't"
of the rules of etiquette, such as: "Don't say 'Me and him went to the
store.'" "Don't use a double negative." ("We haven't got none.") "Don't
use a double comparative." ("Gwynn is a more better batter.") and "Don't
begin a sentence with 'but.'" In no case that I have ever seen does an
error in usage result in misunderstanding, or even in lack of clarity.
The rules of usage describe how educated people are expected, by other
educated people, to speak and write. They are rules of etiquette! And in
some cases, as in the rule about
"but," they are themselves erroneous. As teachers, we have no right
to force them upon our students outside our classrooms.
That does not mean that we should
not teach them, but we should teach them for what they are. They are --
at least those that are valid are -- a feature of formal, educated writing
and speaking. Our job as teachers is to make students aware of them and
to help students see the degree of their validity. Here is where the exception
to "formal focus" comes in. Individually or in small groups, students can
be given one rule of usage with the assignment of reporting on it, either
orally or in writing. A formal class period can be spent either reading
or listening to these reports. The reports should include what the students
found in manuals of style, such as The Chicago Manual of Style,
several of which should be in the school library, if not in the classroom.
(The grammar textbooks in the classroom, by the way, should be burned and
not replaced. Teaching will improve and money will be saved.) Each report
should also include the comments of, let's say, ten educated individuals
briefly interviewed by the students. The students should ask these individuals,
in addition to their level of education and brief job title, if they think
that the rule is valid, why they think so, and how bad they would consider
a violation of the rule to be (on a scale of 10 -- very bad, to 1). They
might also ask if the interviewee perceives a difference between a violation
in something written as opposed to speech.
In addition to getting members
of the community involved in education, these reports, and the class discussions
thereof, will show students (as opposed to being told by the teacher) the
validity of whatever rules the teacher assigns. It is then up to the students
to decide when and if they want to wear them. The classroom, of course,
is at times a formal place. In correcting formal papers, responsible teachers
should mark errors of usage. The degree to which these errors should affect
the grade should be a matter of individual judgment (or departmental policy).
Note, by the way, that KISS
instruction should help with some usage errors. Students who regularly
underline subjects once, and who label complements and identify objects
of prepositions, will soon see part of the problem in "Me and him went
to the store."
Syntactic Errors
Unlike errors of usage, syntactic
errors affect the reader's comprehension of what was written or said. If
a student writes
Thrown from the car, he saw her lying on the
ground.
and means that "he" was thrown from the car, everything is
fine. But if he meant that she was thrown from the car, the sentence does
not say that. In the KISS Approach, the rules of syntax are validated by
our psycholinguistic
model of how the brain processes language. Anything that violates that
model, or that causes the process to crash, is an error. According
to the model, a brain would chunk "Thrown from the car" into one unit,
and then chunk that unit to the next word that makes sense -- which in
this case is "he." This is, of course, close to the traditionalists discussion
of misplaced modifiers, but whereas traditional grammar says "This is the
rule because I say so," the KISS Approach says: "This is the model. The
model makes sense to your brain. Then according to the model and your brain,
the rule has these consequences if you violate it."
I have, by the way, been told
by one college English teacher that many college Freshmen are incapable
of understanding the KISS psycholinguistic model. I don't believe that,
but I do have serious questions about whether or not second graders can
understand it. Part of the teachers' art is in deciding when and how to
introduce the model.
As I hope to show, unlike the
rules of usage, the rules of syntax can always be validated in terms of
what will happen in the readers' brains. And these rules extend even to
such problems as "its" and "it's" and "to" or "too." Consider:
It's raining. |
He wanted to go too. |
Its raining. |
He wanted to go to. |
"It's" means "it is," so "It's raining." is a normal sentence
easily processed. But "its" means "belonging to it." A reader processes
the "Its raining" and expects a verb after it, as in "Its raining made
them cancel the picnic." The period therefore causes confusion --
a crash. "To" always raises the expectation "to what?" "Too" never does.
A person who reads "He wants to go to" is expecting something such as "to
the store," or "to swim." The period thus causes confusion -- either something
is missing, or the word is spelled wrong.
An occasional syntactic error
may be no big deal, but a paper that is salted with them likely presents
pretty barren reading. A reader's brain must use short-term memory not
just for processing sentence structure, but also to keep track of the writer's
thesis, topic sentences, etc. Just as blood rushes to any wound, the focus
of STM shifts to any crash site. If, in the process of reading, one's brain
has to deal with a "to" error, then, in essence, STM is invaded by superfluous
questions -- "Misspelling?" "Something missing? "What's missing?" When
these questions take up slots in a seven-slot STM, something else -- perhaps
the writer's thesis? -- is likely to get shoved out. Simply put, the more
such errors there are in an essay, the less likely the reader is to get
something fruitful out of it.
The way to help students with
syntactic errors is not to present them with a bunch of band-aide rules
that focus on covering the errors. Teach them how sentences and punctuation
are supposed to work -- and teach them by using real texts, including samples
of their own or their peers' writing. Students who know how sentences and
punctuation work, and who can apply that knowledge to their own writing
and reading, do not need to know the names of, or to be given examples
of, errors. The KISS Approach provides students with the instruction that
they need.
Errors of Pronoun Reference
Because the KISS Approach concerns
how words syntactically function in sentences, and because pronouns can
function in any way that nouns can, KISS pays only minor attention to pronouns.
As in many other aspects of the teaching of grammar, both too much and
not enough are being done.
I can't figure out why students
need to know the names of so many types of pronouns -- relative, demonstrative,
indefinite, interrogative, possessive, reflexive. Year after year we have
been trying to cram all these names into students' brains, and the students
forget all of it, including the one group they should remember, the
personal. The personal pronouns involve simple distinctions:
First person -- |
the person speaking/writing
("I," "me," "my," "mine," etc.) |
Second person -- |
The person spoken/written to
("you," "your," etc.) |
Third person -- |
The person spoken/written about
("he," "she," "it," "they," etc.) |
Clarifying these distinctions for students may help those
who have troubles with shifts in person. ("We went to the park. There you
saw big elephants.") We cannot, however, as we now apparently do, just
teach these distinctions and forget them. If we do, then students will
forget them likewise. It isn't difficult to work these terms into assignments
two or three times a year, just enough to keep students from forgetting.
("In your journal for this week, write to someone you haven't seen in a
while. Use and underline second person pronouns.")
I must admit that I myself did
not discuss personal pronouns with my students -- that is until two or
three came back to report they were having problems. In fields such as
Human Services and many of the technologies, first person is verboten.
The instructors, incorrectly assuming that we English teachers are doing
our jobs, simply told students not to use first person in their papers.
The students didn't understand, used first person, and either got lowered
grades or got their papers handed back to them to be rewritten. This is,
of course, an excellent opportunity to deal with a question of usage --
in some contexts (for example, the autobiographical), first person is required;
in others, it is optional, and in others prohibited.
The other undertaught aspect
of pronouns is number. I'm still surprised that so few of my college Freshmen
know what the term means in a grammatical context, especially since the
concept is not that difficult. English currently distinguishes between
one ("she," "he," "it) and more than one ("they"). Grammatically, and this,
of course, relates to subject/verb agreement as well, "singular"
refers to words that denote one, whereas "plural" refers to words that
denote more than one. Part of the problem is that students see the three
terms ("number," "singular," and "plural") as isolated, rather than
conceptualizing "singular" and "plural" as the two subdivisions of "number."
Errors in agreement can present psycholinguistic processing hazards --
"One can see their own reflections in the pond." But, more importantly,
the distinction between one and more than one has major logical, philosophical,
and psychological implications. A writer who can't keep track of whether
he is referring to one or to more than one is not thinking very clearly.
And if the writer isn't thinking, why should a reader bother to read what
he wrote?
The KISS Approach, it should
be clear, will not solve all of students' problems with usage and pronoun
reference. Some time will have to be spent on usage, preferably, as noted
above, individually or in small groups, and students should be taught a
few things about pronouns. But because it focusses on meaning, and because
it focusses on the meaning and function of every word in every sentence,
the KISS Approach will help. The student who wrote "We went to the park.
There you saw big elephants." doesn't really need an explanation of grammatical
person; he simply needs to be asked, "Why should I see big elephants because
you went to the park?"
Improving Writing and Avoiding
Errors
from the Perspective of the
Five KISS Levels
KISS Level One --
Basic Subjects, Verbs, Complements,
Adjectives, Adverbs, and Prepositional
Phrases.
Improving Writing
Teachers often ask students to use "string"
verbs, but this instruction is not helpful to students who cannot identify
verbs in the first place. In teaching students to identify subjects and
verbs in KISS Level 1.1, KISS includes an exercise on filling in
the blanks with verbs. The idea, of course, is both to help students learn
to identify verbs and to help them find stronger and more interesting verbs.
This exercise, which is repeated in many of the "Practice/Application"
sections, works best in a classroom situation where students can share
and discuss the verbs that they have used to fill in the blanks.
Some teachers tell students not to use adjectives--to
use forceful nouns instead. This is usually meaningless instruction --
most of my college Freshmen can identify neither adjectives or nouns. In
KISS Level 1.3, students learn to identify both adjectives and adverbs.
Once they can do this, they can explore the question of adjectives vs.
forceful nouns for themselves.
KISS applications to writing really
begin to kick in at Level 1.4 (Compounding). In the 1960's and 70's
Kellogg Hunt, Roy O'Donnell, and Walter Loban made major breakthroughs
with the concept of "syntactic maturity." It is obvious that eighth graders
write longer, more complex sentences than do fourth graders, but until
Hunt's research validated the "T-unit," there was no accurate way to measure
such maturity. The "T-unit" turns out to be the KISS "main clause." It
is, in other words, a main S/V/C pattern and all the words that chunk to
it, including any subordinate clauses.
This research resulted in the 1980's fascination
with sentence-combining exercises, but far too many of those exercises
were aimed at increasing the types of constructions that students used
in their writing. Thus second graders were asked to combine sentences with
appositives. The students, of course, were not taught what appositives
are, but trying to get second graders to use appositives is a bad idea
(for reasons too complex to go into here). The point here is that if you
start to analyze randomly selected sentences, you will probably be surprised
by the frequency with which writers use compounds (especially compound
verbs and compound complements) as they write longer sentences.
The exercises on compounding do, of course, give
students examples of such compounding, but teachers might also want to
stress how compounds can improve writing by replacing an abstract word
with more concrete examples. Instead of "We played games," a better sentence
would be something like "We played baseball, basketball, and soccer." Although
the exercises in KISS Level 1.4 do not currently stress this, the "Practice/Application"
sections each include an exercise on abstract and concrete words. (In the
"Practice/Application" section for KISS Level One, this exercise is on
"Common" vs. "Proper" nouns, but are not "common" nouns abstractions and
"proper" nouns concrete?)
Like many of the later KISS sections, Level
1.4 includes both a combining and a decombining exercise. (The noted educational
psychologists Piaget and Vygotsky both claimed that cognitive mastery includes
the ability to reverse a mental operation. Thus KISS uses de-combining
exercises almost as often as combining exercises. Also like many of the
other subsections, KISS Level 1.4 includes a writing exercise. In this
section it is simply "Write a sentence that has three or more verbs for
one subject. Write another sentence that has four or more complements for
one verb." Teachers, of course, may want to adapt this exercise by, for
example, having students include such sentences in something that they
themselves write.
I have, by the way, heard some teachers make
fun of having students use specific constructions in their own writing.
They remind me of people who make fun of something that they do not understand.
Perhaps they are examples of such people? Surely, having students use a
specific grammatical construction in something they are writing does no
harm -- if students are studying that construction. Indeed, this type of
exercise is precisely what many other teachers call for when then claim
that grammar should be taught only in the context of writing. (The problem
with this side of the balance, of course, is that the students are generally
not taught how to identify grammatical constructions.)
KISS Level 1.5, the addition of prepositional
phrases, has three possible objectives. Teachers can, of course, stop at
the first objective, which is simply the students' ability to identify
such phrases. The second objective is to have the students see how such
phrases chunk to the rest of the sentence (usually as adjectives or adverbs).
But the third objective is the most important for writing -- the logic
of prepositional phrases.
In the "complete" books, level 1.5 includes
two exercises on the logic of prepositional phrases. The KISS approach
to logic is based on David Hume's concepts (See the essay.), one of which
is extension in time and space. Many students write narratives that take
place in a vacuum -- they include few, in any details of where and when
the story happened. Prepositional phrases are a major way of adding such
details. Thus, once students can identify prepositional phrases, and can
see how these phrases add such details, they find it much easier to include
such details in their writing.
Another important stylistic aspect of prepositional
phrases is as sentence openers. I once received an e-mail from a parent
who was very upset that his children's teacher was encouraging them to
begin sentences with prepositional phrases. He was sure that he had been
taught that sentences should not begin with prepositional phrases. He was
probably confused by the nonsense rule (frequently taught) that sentences
should not begin with "But," but given that students are not usually taught
to identify prepositional phrases in the first place, his confusion is
understandable. KISS does not include any exercises specifically devoted
to this question, but students doing KISS analysis exercises will frequently
see prepositional phrases are the beginnings of sentences. Teachers who
want to emphasize varying sentence openings should find it easy to do so
once their students can identify prepositional phrases in the first place.
(The same is true for subordinate clauses, verbals, etc.)
KISS Level 1.6 is devoted to "Case, Number,
and Tense." From the perspective of improving writing, most of this section
enables students to understand "error" questions such as subject/verb agreement,
tense shifts, etc., but the use of pronouns, especially personal pronouns,
involves numerous stylistic questions. KISS itself does not address many
of these questions, but it does enable students to understand them. In
college, for example, the use of first person pronouns in actually prohibited
in some papers in some disciplines. As a college writing instructor, I
have found that the students' basic problem is that when they are told
not to use first person, they have no idea what "first person" is. KISS,
in other words, primarily addresses pronouns as a vocabulary question.
Avoiding Errors
Native speakers rarely, if ever,
use simple prepositional phrases incorrectly, and when they do, the problem
is usually one of modification. One student, for example, wrote "At the
age of thirteen, my father obtained custody of me." To help students avoid
such errors, teachers should probably NOT focus on them. As students place
parentheses around prepositional phrases and draw arrows to the word each
modifies, errors such as this one will become apparent to the students
themselves, especially in view of the psycholinguistic
model of how the brain processes language.
Subject/Verb Agreement Errors
Prepositional phrases do often
contribute to errors in combination with other constructions. The most
widely recognized of these is the slipped pattern in which the object of
a preposition is confused with the subject of a verb, thereby resulting
in a subject/verb agreement error, as in "Neither of these are very difficult."
This is one of the reasons why KISS addresses prepositional phrases.
With prepositional phrases neatly tucked in parentheses, students find
it much easier to recognize subjects and verbs. Once they recognize them,
many students automatically fix agreement errors; some students, however,
do need a little prompting.
Year after year, students are "taught"
the rule that subjects and verbs must agree in "number" -- if the subject
is plural, its verb must be plural; if the subject is singular, its verb
must be singular. This is, perhaps, the most destructive "instruction"
that ever occurs in our classrooms. It is destructive because it is meaningless,
hence boring, and it teaches students that grammar itself is, for them,
a meaningless morass of menacing mistakes. In other words, it teaches students
to tune out.
As noted in the discussion (above)
of prepositional phrases, once students can identify subjects and verbs,
they can usually fix problems in subject / verb agreement. Their problem
is that typical instruction in grammar has never taught them how to identify
subjects and verbs in the first place. KISS very directly addresses this
problem in that students learn to identify subjects and verbs in Level
One, and they will continue to identify subjects and verbs in every
sentence that they analyze. As they learn to recognize subjects and verbs,
in any text, including their own writing, the rule about agreement in "number"
makes sense. In fact, they probably do not even need the rule since, as
native speakers of English, they have already taught themselves that subjects
and verbs should so agree.
"Its" and "It's," "Their" and "They're" and Their Relatives
Magazine and newspaper articles
about grammatical errors are fairly common. In them, "its" and "it's,"
"their" and "they're" and similar homonyms usually rank near the top of
the list of errors. We can, of course, remind students that "it's," with
the apostrophe, means "it is," but the larger problem here is that many
students, including those who are most likely to have problems here, read
words and not sentences. Thus our "instruction" is not easily applied to
their writing. In the KISS Approach, however, students will be underlining
hundreds, more likely thousands of subject / verb patterns. Among them,
they will quickly discover "it's" and its relatives -- or they won't find
them and thus realize that they do not have a subject/verb. Or they will
find them, where they don't belong, and realize that they wrote "That is
it is doghouse."
The "Of" and "Have" Problem
A student once complained to
the President of the college I was teaching at. He sent the President a
letter in which he wrote, "I should of passed this course." The President
called me in to share a laugh. Most experienced writing teachers realize
that this problem is the result of students mastering the language orally,
rather than through a lot of reading. The only effective way to eliminate
the problem, however, is to teach students that "of" is a preposition and
"have" is a verb. But even this instruction is useless unless students
analyze the prepositional phrases and S/V/C patterns in numerous sentences.
And that, of course, is precisely what the KISS Approach has them do.
The Logic of Complements
College professors in electronics,
automotive, and several other disciplines have complained to me that their
students do not answer their questions. "I ask the students why, and the
students tell me what. I ask the students under what conditions, and the
students tell me what. I ask the students when, and the students tell me
what. Whatever my question, the students answer it as a "what" question."
In KISS, students learn to identify complements by asking the question
"Whom or what?" after the verb. The question cannot be "when?" "why?" "how?"
or any question other than "whom or what?" Every semester, I tell my students
what I have just written. And every semester I am amazed at the trouble
that some students have in limiting the question to "whom or what?" I also
tell the students that, in learning how to identify complements, they should
also be learning to stop and think about the questions that their instructors
are asking them. If they answer a "how" question as if it were a "what"
question, they are probably guaranteeing that what could have been a "A"
instead comes back with no better than a "C" on it.
S / V / Predicate Noun Logic
The KISS Approach to teaching complements
focuses on meaning. Thus one finds the complement by asking "Verb + What?,"
and if the verb implies equality, and the subject and the answer to the
question are in any way equal, then the complement is a predicate noun.
One student wrote:
Often the practice rooms are the only time one can
be alone.
The verb "are" implies equality, but a room is not a time.
Although some people would consider this to be a minor error, it is a clear
reflection that 1) the student wasn't thinking, or 2) the student cannot
distinguish time from space. This error is far more common than one might
expect. It has not been discussed in any detail because traditional grammars
don't have an effective way of describing it, and even if they could describe
it, they cannot address it effectively because they do not teach students
to identify S/V/C patterns. As I try to suggest in the section on "Syntax
and the Logic of David Hume," the concept of identity (What is equal
to what?) is essential in life, and it is crucial in the technical fields
that many students go into. And the S/V/PN pattern is a fundamental way
of expressing such an equality.
Students who have trouble handling
the pattern often say things they do not mean. In technical writing, this
causes extremely serious errors, but even in everyday writing it can lead
the reader astray. The problem, moreover, gets worse as students embed
more and more constructions into a single clause. Thus one student wrote:
The taste of a sizzling foot-long hotdog coated
with tangy sauerkraut with mounds of pickle relish is a typical snack when
accompanied by a tall, chilled paper cup of Coke.
Having read that sentence, I was basically useless as a reader
for the rest of the student's essay. I was too distracted, wondering how
a "taste" could be a "snack." I tried to imagine a "taste" as a snack,
but most people I know want more than just a taste when they have a snack.
Perhaps the student was writing for ghosts? My point here is not to make
fun of the student, but rather to suggest that such errors will lead thoughtful
readers off track and into a series of questions that the writer had no
intention of evoking.
By teaching students to identify
S/V/PN patterns, the KISS Approach enables students to recognize such errors
in their writing and shows them how to fix them. In this case, the meaningful
subject is in the prepositional phrase, and thus the sentence can be fixed
by changing it to "A tasty, sizzling, foot-long hotdog ... is a typical
snack...."
KISS Level Two -- Expanding
the Basic Concepts
Level Two
is where KISS really begins to differ from most grammar textbooks. Its
primary focus is to help students find S/V/C patterns and prepositional
phrases in real texts. Thus it deals with questions that most grammar textbooks
ignore. For example, Level 2.1.6 teaches students how to distinguish finite
verbs from verbals. Typical textbooks teach students what subjects and
verbs are, but they use very basic, sanitized exercises. When dealing with
real texts, however, students will find sentences such as:
a.) Swimming is good exercise.
b.) They saw her swimming in the
lake.
c.) Bob went to the park to swim.
"Swimming" and "to swim" are verbs,
and given what is taught in most textbooks, students will want to underline
these words twice, only to find out that they are wrong. This is the type
of problem that Level Two helps students with. As a result, level two does
not introduce new questions of writing or of errors. Note, however, that
as students do exercises in KISS Level Two, they will continue to analyze
real sentences from randomly selected texts. Thus all of the items discussed
in Level One can continue to be a focus in Level Two.
Improving Writing
The one thing about writing style that is introduced
in Level Two is Level 2.1.2 -- Varied Positions in the S/V/C Pattern. Many
students are surprised to see that complements can come before subjects
and/or verbs, as in "Him I know." For those of us who have analyzed texts,
this is a very simple idea, but it is a revelation to some students, and,
of course, the varied patterns change the emphasis and variety in the sentence
structure.
Avoiding Errors
Here again, not much new is introduced in Level
Two, except for some teachers. The subjunctive mood (Level 2.1.7) is introduced
here primarily to make sure that teachers do not make errors by marking
a sentence such as "I wish he were here" as having an agreement error in
"he were." This material, of course, also enables teachers to explain to
students why "he were" is not an error in such sentences.
KISS Level
Three (Clauses)
Improving Writing (and Reading)
The February 1984 issue of English
Journal, the dominant publication for high school English teachers,
includes an
article by Trevor Gambell. Gambell claimed that his research showed
that many students have problems with exam questions that include subordinate
clauses. Apparently, many students have problems distinguishing the main
idea in such sentences. Gambell did not, however, conclude that we should
teach these students how to understand sentences that include subordinate
clauses. He concluded that exams should be written in simpler sentences.
Need I comment on this? If you work with KISS Level Three, you may conclude
that it is the most important of all KISS levels. Indeed, KISS Levels one
and two may be seen as preparing students for KISS Level Three. A "clause"
is an S/V/C pattern and all the words that chunk to it. Students who have
mastered KISS Levels One and Two should have relatively few problems with
clauses.
Compound Main Clauses
The section below on "avoiding errors"
explains why comma-splices and run-ons are serious errors. But if you look
at why students make these errors in the first place, you will probably
find, as I did, that students often use either no punctuation (or just
a comma) between main clauses because they sense a logical connection between
the clauses so joined. In other words, they have not been taught about
how experienced writers use semicolons, colons, or dashes to connect compound
main clauses.
Although some writers apparently
use these three punctuation marks interchangeably to separate main clauses,
many writers use the semicolon to imply or reinforce a contrast:
(a.) He went swimming. She did the dishes.
(b.) He went swimming, and she did the dishes.
(c.) He went swimming; she did the dishes.
Most readers will interpret both (a.) and (b.) as two equally
important statements of fact. The semicolon in (c.) however, will lead
many readers to look for an implied contrast -- which they will probably
find. Example (c.) implies that he's out there having fun, whereas she
is stuck working in the kitchen.
Colons and dashes, on the other
hand, usually suggest that whatever follows them will be a restatement
(often in more detail) of the first main clause:
It was a nice day: it was sunny with a light
breeze.
It was a nice day -- it was sunny with a light breeze.
The colon tends to be used in formal writing; the dash, in
informal. By teaching students how to identify main clauses in real texts,
and by giving students these generalizations about the semicolon, colon,
and dash, KISS not only addresses two of the most complained about writing
errors, it shows students how to change the negative of the error into
a logical positive of their style.
In the "complete" workbooks,
KISS begins Level Three with exercises on compound main clauses. Four of
the nine exercises in Level 3.1.1 focus on the logic and punctuation of
these clauses.
Adding Subordinate Clauses
Having learned the concept of
"clause" in Level 3.1.1, students add subordinate clauses to their analytical
toolbox in Level 3.1.2 Stylistically, this level is probably the most important
in KISS. Among other things, it includes having students rewrite main clauses
as subordinate and subordinate as main:
He went swimming while she did the dishes.
While he went swimming, she did the dishes.
The stylistics of subordinate clauses is debated; the teaching
of the stylistics of clauses should be more so.
MIMC
For example, if you press most
grammar teachers, they will admit that in most well-written sentences,
the main idea is in the main clause S/V/C pattern (MIMC). That idea, however,
is too simple for many teachers of future grammar teachers. They want to
focus on the exceptions to the rule. Admittedly, numerous exceptions can
be found. But if you talk with many middle school teachers, they will probably
tell you that their students often have trouble getting their main idea
into the main clause S/V/C pattern. The problem with all of this yakety-yak
is that textbooks do not teach students how to identify clauses in the
first place. KISS does, and because it does, it can introduce students
to the questions -- Is the main idea of most sentences in the main S/V/C
pattern? Should it be? Why, or why not? KISS, in other words, gives students
the tools they will need to discuss these questions intelligently and to
come to their own conclusions.
Branching
The same is true in regard to
left-branching and right-branching subordination. "Left-branching" means
that the subordinate construction comes before the main subject and verb;
"right-branching" means that it comes after:
Left: While she did the dishes, he went swimming.
Right: He went swimming while she did the dishes.
What precedes a sentence has, of course, some influence on
whether a writer will use left or right branching, but often writers unconsciously
make that decision for other reasons. What troubles me is that some writing
instructors push students (who usually cannot identify clauses in the first
place) toward either left or right branching.
I admire, for example, the work
of Francis Christiansen, but I am annoyed when he pushes students toward
right branching constructions, apparently because most writers use it more
than left-branching. (That most people do it is not really a good reason.)
On the other hand, I admire Walker Gibson, who, in Tough, Sweet and Stuffy,
suggests that left-branching implies a more organized brain. His argument
is that in order to write a left-branching sentence, the writer has to
have the whole idea organized in his or her mind. Otherwise, the writer
would not know what the subordinate clause is subordinate to. Gibson goes
on to note that right-branching is comparatively easy--and it can reflect
thoughts that are simply tacked on to what has just been said. Personally,
I have no position in this debate. In KISS Grammar students can explore
this question and make up their own minds.
The Logic of Subordinate Clauses
In addition to focus (MIMC) and
branching, KISS Level 3.1.2 includes exercises on the logic of subordinate
clauses.
"He went swimming while she did the dishes.'
has a focus on "He went," and a logical connection of time.
"He went swimming because she did the dishes.' has a
focus on "He went," and a logical connection of cause/effect.
"She did the dishes, so he went swimming." has a focus
on "She did," and a logical connection of result (a variant of cause/effect).
Some students, of course, unconsciously feel these distinctions
and automatically use the version that reflects their intended focus and
logical connection. But many students do not get it. Indeed, they
have no idea that such "subtle" distinctions can be made. KISS exercises
on the logic of subordinate clauses not only help them realize more possibilities
than they previously saw, the exercises give them practice in manipulating
clause structures.
KISS Level 3.1.3 focuses on embedded
subordinate clauses. Actually all subordinate clauses are embedded in a
main clause, but at KISS Level 3.1.3 students explore sentences that have
subordinate clauses within subordinate clauses. "Is that possible?" some
students usually ask. My response is, "Don't ask me. See for yourself."
For some students the embedding of subordinate clauses within subordinate
clauses is no revelation. But even for many college students, the analysis
of such sentences raises serious stylistic questions. Is heavy embedding
good? Or bad? Our psycholinguistic model provides a context for answering
that question, but what yardstick can one use to determine how deep embedding
should be -- or how long main clauses should be?
Although the exercises in the
KISS levels include many application exercises, some "applications" can
evolve into major research projects. This is definitely the case with the
questions of how deep embedding should be and of how long main clauses
should be. KISS approaches these two questions (and many others) in statistical
exercises in the "Practice/Application" books that follow each KISS
Level. (Note that these "Practice/Application" books also include additional
exercises on the logic and punctuation of main clauses and on the logic
of subordinate clauses.)
Statistical Stylistics
As noted above, Kellogg Hunt
did some widely respected research on syntactic maturity with his basic
yardstick, the "T-unit." And, for Hunt, the "T-unit" was exactly the KISS
concept of the main clause -- a "T-unit" is a main clause defined as including
all its subordinate clauses and other subordinate constructions. Thus the
main clause gives students a basic yardstick with which they can do math
in English class. The results of Hunt's and others' research is included
in KISS Level 6.5 "Statistical Stylistics." There are some serious questions
about it, but basically it suggests that third and fourth graders average
around eight words per main clause; fifth, sixth, and seventh graders,
around nine; eighth and ninth graders around ten; tenth and eleventh graders
around eleven; and twelfth graders average between thirteen and fourteen.
Profession writers, they claim, average around twenty. My own college Freshmen
usually average between fifteen and sixteen.
Although these numbers
offer a general sense of the statistical norm, within KISS grammar students
who can identify main and subordinate clauses can analyze their own writing
(and that of others) and use their own studies to determine how long, on
average, and how deeply embedded subordinate clauses (on average) writing
should have. Determining words per main clause, for example, is simply
a matter of counting the words in the analyzed text. (Most word processors
will give students that number so that they do not even have to count the
words.) In KISS, we put a vertical line after each main clause. Students,
therefore, only have to count the number of vertical lines that they have
put in the analyzed text. If the divide the number of words in the text
by the number of vertical lines (main clauses) they have arrived at Hunt's
"words per T-unit" or, in KISS, "words per main clause."
If they are working in a classroom
context, they do not even need all those statistics cited above. Teachers
can arrive at an even better norm simply by averaging the averages of the
students in the class to arrive at a "norm" for the students in the class.
Almost two decades ago, Robert Boynton, of Boynton/Cook Publishers, was
interested in publishing a book about KISS Grammar. But he adamantly objected
to statistical statistics. Stupidly, I insisted on including them. I say
"stupidly" because I could have agreed, published a book with a well-known
publisher, and then written about statistical stylistics in articles or
in another book.
I think I understand why Mr. Boynton
objected -- I did a poor job of explaining the importance of the psycholinguistic
model. At that time, sentence-combining was the rage -- students should
write longer, more complicated sentences. Mr. Boynton, I think, did not
agree with that, and he was afraid that any move toward statistical analysis
would promote the American fallacy -- bigger and longer is better.
The psycholinguistic model,
however, puts an upper "limit" on sentence length and complexity. Bigger
is not better if most readers will have trouble reading the text because
of its long, complex sentences. At the other end, of course, readers will
perceive a college student as immature if he or she is writing, on average,
simple main clauses that average ten words. In other words, I regularly
suggest to students that they want to be somewhere close to the norm. True,
some high school (and even some college) English teachers give high grades
for long, complex sentences. But most college professors don't. What they
care about is the content. Note that I'm not saying that length and complexity
of sentence structure it totally irrelevant; I'm saying that length and
complexity for the sake of length and complexity often distracts college
professors from what they are really interested in -- the content of the
paper.
Statistical analysis can be
connected with another important aspect of writing -- the writer's intended
readers. Would it be surprising to learn that papers written for college
courses typically include more words per average main clause than do articles
in popular magazines? How about professional journals? It would not be
difficult to divide a class into groups and to have one group analyze passages
from different magazines, another group passages from newspapers, etc.
Indeed, the students might even be asked to write papers that explain what
texts they analyzed and what they discovered. These papers could then be
used by students in later years as sources of discussion and further study
of the typical sentences in different types of texts.
From the students' point of
view, an obviously more practical series of studies could focus on writing
samples from the assessment documents of various state Departments of Education.
Many states have evaluated examples of students' writing available on the
internet These usually include the writing prompts, directions, and criteria
for the evaluations. Studying these essays is obviously good preparation
for students, but the KISS approach enables students to extend the study
of these essays not only to questions of errors, but also to the statistical
analysis of the various samples. Do the eighth grade essays that get the
highest evaluations also have the highest average of words per main clause?
Do those with the lowest evaluations have the lowest? The KISS site already
includes some of these samples, statistically analyzed. But having the
students do the analysis themselves would probably be more convincing for
the students. And even if these studies suggest that bigger is better,
the "bigger" would be, as noted above, a statistical norm appropriate for
eighth graders.
If you do have students do statistical
studies, you will probably decide that the process itself is more important
than the numerical results. Counting subordinate clauses forces students
to look closely at subordinate clauses. Students particularly find analyzing
samples of their own writing interesting, especially if it is being done
against the framework of a norm, whether the norm be their own class, a
set of papers from state standards, or professional writing. When a student
finds no subordinate clauses in his or her own writing, but is convinced
by the norm that most of his or her peers do have such clauses, the student
is much more motivated to do sentence-combining exercises. If, on the other
hand, a student finds his or her clauses too long and too heavily embedded,
the student, in light of the KISS psycholinguistic model, is usually easily
persuaded to do some de-combining exercises.
But there is more. KISS statistical
projects enable teachers to introduce many of the intrinsic problems of
statistical conclusions. How many individual samples have to be analyzed
and then averaged for a "norm" to be credible? Why are definitions of terms
important? The credibility of some of the professional studies that followed
Hunt's is very weak because the researchers for a vaguely explained reason
counted adverbial clauses of cause as separate main clauses. Another question
that students will run into is how to count constructions that can be explained
in more than one way. And, if the samples are hand-written by students,
what does one do with "garbles." (Hunt and most of those who followed him
defined "garbles" as words or phrases that were illegible.) The point here
is that KISS can bring the essence of the scientific method (inductive
conclusions from individual observations) and the whole questions of the
methods and credibility of statistical research into the classroom. And
it does this in the context of the students' research into the way that
their own brains process language.
Avoiding Errors
Some students, especially those
who learned the language orally and did not do a lot of reading, make frequent
errors related to clause boundaries. These errors are serious because the
clause is the primary unit in the process of reading. As words enter a
reader's short-term memory, they are chunked to each other until everything
that is supposed to go together is eventually chunked to the S/V/C pattern
of a main clause. At the end of that clause, the writer should have used
punctuation which signals a dump to long-term memory. If parts of the pattern
are missing, or if the punctuation is missing or incorrect, the reader
becomes confused. Although students do not need to know the names for various
errors, teachers probably do so that they can understand the nature of
the students' problems and determine what, if anything, to do about them.
Because clause-boundary errors are a focus of "Cobweb
Corner," my research area, the following brief explanations include
links to the
relevant discussions
in that research material.
To my knowledge, almost nothing
that we are currently doing in our schools helps students with these problems.
The reasons for that are simple. The sentences in the exercises that students
are given to work with are too simple, much simpler than the sentences
that the students themselves often write (and thus have problems with).
Then there are the teachers who tell students to put a period wherever
they would make a "long" pause in speech. That advice is simply stupid.
"Because we talk in fragments."
The KISS Approach definitely
helps students because as they analyze real sentences from randomly selected
texts, they come to learn how sentences -- and punctuation -- work. We
need to understand, moreover, that we cannot expect immediate results.
Under pressure, as in in-class writing, students will still make mistakes,
and as for out-of-class writing, we need to teach students the difference
between editing and revising. Then we need to force them first to revise
and then to edit.
Fragments
As the name suggests, fragments
are parts of sentence patterns that are punctuated as complete sentences.
Often, fragments are the result of an overload of the writer's short-term
memory (STM). With STM overloaded, the inexperienced writer simply puts
down a period (or some other main-clause-ending punctuation mark), and
then writes the rest of the main clause (often a subordinate clause) as
a separate sentence. In the KISS Approach, the teacher's job is to point
out to students that the fragment can probably be connected to the sentence
either before or after it.
The problem with fragments appears
to be most common in grades seven through nine. For anyone familiar with
the research of Hunt, O'Donnell,
and Loban, this is not surprising
because these researchers have convincingly demonstrated that these years
are the period of most intense growth in the use of subordinate clauses.
Unfortunately, most English teachers are not familiar with this research,
and even more unfortunately, almost no thought has been given to its implications.
It is quite possible, for example, that the current attempts to "help"
these students actually do more harm than good.
We know, for example, that as
young children we all said such things as "I cutted the paper," and "Turn
on it" (for "Turn it on.") Even if no one EVER corrected us, we all learned
the correct forms. But just as the learning of irregular verb forms is
part of natural syntactic development, so is the growth of subordinate
clauses. There are, of course, two significant differences here. Errors
such as "I cutted" are made orally. Children will hear the correct forms
and naturally assimilate them. Clause boundary errors are all graphic.
Students who read a lot tend to assimilate the correct forms as they read.
The other difference is that
subordinate clause growth occurs well into the school years, when teachers
feel that they have to "do something," especially because these errors
are heavily penalized in the evaluation of state-wide writing exams. But
instruction, by its very nature, is an intervention into the "natural."
And what we tend to do is to impress upon students -- by the very fact
that we give them exercises to avoid fragments -- that there is a problem,
but the exercises we give them do not work -- for the reasons stated above.
By interfering, in other words, we might well be making the problem worse
-- adding both anxiety and lack of clarity to it.
I would like to see a lot more
research done on actual students' writing to determine the nature of students'
fragments, on their relative frequency (per main clause), and on the grade
levels at which they occur. I'm wondering if fragments that occur in students'
writing before students study KISS Level Three should simply be ignored.
(Teachers might correct them in students' writing, but not count off for
them or do any instruction about them.) In the KISS Curriculum, students
will be learning to analyze the clauses in their own writing. In this process,
they will begin to recognize any fragments in their writing, and, as suggested
above, they will have a clear context for understanding the problem --
and for fixing it.
We should not leave the question
of fragments without noting that some fragments
are totally acceptable. Currently, instruction is vague about
which are acceptable and which are not, but the KISS Approach here, as
almost always, relies on the psycholinguistic
model of how the brain processes language: a fragment that might cause
a crash is bad; one that probably will not, is not only acceptable but
sometimes a sign of good writing. Good fragments usually, but not always,
appear at the beginning of a paragraph, where they establish a topic or
attitude that is developed in the paragraph, or at the end of a paragraph,
where the reader can obviously see the coming paragraph break and will
therefore not expect a completion to the fragment. (Click
here for the Cobweb Corner discussion of fragments.)
Comma-splices
and Run-ons
Comma-splices and
run-ons are related in that two main clauses are joined by only a comma
(CS) or the second main clause runs into the first with no punctuation
between them (RO). These errors create the exact opposite of the problem
created by a fragment. Instead of being directed to dump to long-term memory
with only a partial pattern in STM, the reader has a complete pattern in
STM and starts trying to chunk the words from the next pattern into the
previous one. Because they don't chunk, a crash may occur. I say "may"
because, as most grammar textbooks state, comma-splices are acceptable
if the main clauses are short. Unfortunately, they do not say how long
"short" can be.
KISS, relying on the psycholinguistic
model, states that if the intended readers can be expected to have no problems
processing the sentence, then the splices should be considered acceptable.
Parallel constructions, for example, make sentences easier to process,
and adults can process longer sentences than can fifth graders. This still
leaves the question with a subjective answer. The KISS Approach would settle
any questionable case in the student's favor, provided, of course, that
the student has been taught through the KISS Approach, and therefore understands
that the splices might cause readers to crash.
The opening
of Dickens' A Tale of Two Cities, by the way, was included
among the exercises precisely because of his use of comma-splices and parallel
constructions. For the Cobweb Corner
discussion of comma-splices and run-ons, click here.
Incomplete
Subordination
I haven't had the time or opportunity
to collect a lot of examples of it, but teachers should expect to see cases
of incomplete subordination:
Although the author
Kent Scheidegger of the essay "Habeas Corpus is Abused by Convicts" relays
many good examples of the abuse of this procedure, but the fallacies in
which the author commits weakens his essay and argument dramatically.
[In this case, the writer has subordinated the first clause
with "although," but has retained the "but" that would join two main clauses.]
Incomplete subordination probably results from one of two
things (or perhaps a combination of both). For one, the student may be
in the process of mastering subordinate clauses. Part of that process
involves reducing a main clause in a compound sentence into a subordinate
clause. In the example, the student made it half-way. The other cause is
that the main clause that the student is attempting to write is beyond
his (or her) STM processing capacity. As a result, the first part of the
sentence, once written, gets pushed out of STM, and the sentence then,
to use Mina Shaughnessy's
term, "slips" into a different pattern. Shaughnessy's Errors and Expectations,
by the way, should be read by every teacher of grammar and/or writing.
From the perspective of errors,
KISS Level Three, the mastery of clauses, is the most important -- as long
as the students are also well-grounded in the psycholinguistic model. The
model addresses the question of modification, and if the students understand
it, they will be able to address the errors that are discussed under Levels
Four and Five without even studying the concepts for those levels.
KISS Level Four (Verbals
-- Gerunds, Gerundives, and Infinitives)
Compared
to KISS Level Three, KISS Levels Four and Five add only a few grammatical
constructions that seriously affect the style of writing. Teachers who
want to emphasize the connections between sentence-structure and writing,
can, of course, use additional exercises from KISS Levels One through Three
or from the "Practice/Application" books.
Improving Writing
Of the three types of verbal,
gerunds and infinitives tend to develop naturally, and their use depends
on the topic the students are writing about. They do not need any
special focus. They do "grow," but they tend to do so by having additional
constructions added to (embedded in) them. Thus "We like playing baseball,"
may grow to "We like playing baseball with our friends from Dover High
School on Sunday afternoons when there is no interesting Orioles game on
T.V." KISS does include "free" sentence-combining exercise (in the Practice/Application"
books that can be used to encourage this type of growth, but special focus
on it is probably not needed.
Gerundives are a different question.
Most gerundives can be viewed as reduced subordinate clauses:
Many children like the Harry Potter book. These books
were written by J. K. Rowling.
Many children like the Harry Potter books that were written
by J. K. Rowling.
Many children like the Harry Potter books written by
J. K. Rowling.
In the "complete" books, one exercise in Level Four is
devoted to this type of sentence-manipulation exercise.
Avoiding Errors
Students have few problems in
using verbals correctly, but the two problems that some students do have
are fairly serious.
Misplaced or Dangling
Modifiers
"Misplaced" and/or "dangling"
modifiers are errors that frustrate many high school teachers (and college
professors). They are also known as "dangling and/or misplaced participles"
because most grammars don't make the distinction that KISS does between
"participle" as form and "gerundive" as function.
One student wrote, for example,
"Being of an impulsive nature, my mother often accompanies me when purchasing
clothing. " In that sentence, the gerundive "Being" chunks to "mother,"
but that is not what the writer meant. Her mother accompanies the writer
to restrain the writer's impulsiveness. Although some English teachers
claim that these errors are not important, they can be very important.
Assume, for example, that the following sentence appears in a police report:
Thrown from the car, he saw her lying on the ground.
If "he" was the one thrown from the car, the sentence is
fine, but what if she is the one who was thrown? The legal case may be
entirely different. And writers who cannot control gerundives (such as
the writer of the sentence about impulsiveness) may write this sentence
and mean that she was the one who was thrown.
Obviously not many dangling
or misplaced gerundives will have serious legal consequences, but they
can seriously affect communication, especially when they result in a humorous
sentence that the writer did not intend:
Our stomachs were full of butterflies wondering whether,
after all this work, we could pull this performance off as a success.
As I tried to read the rest of this student's paper, I
could not get these wondering butterflies out of my mind. I pictured them
fluttering up to each other, hovering, and chit-chatting -- "What might
go wrong?" "Will we succeed?" Yellow ones, tan ones, white ones! Butterflies.
All wondering. As I wondered about the wondering butterflies, I probably
did not give the paper a fair reading. I was too distracted. And that is,
perhaps, the primary problem with misplaced modifiers. It is the writer's
responsibility to control the structure of sentences. When writers do not,
and readers get distracted, whose fault is it?
In order to help students avoid
errors such as these, KISS focuses on the adjectival function of gerundives
-- Gerundives "always" function as adjectives. Actually,
most gerundives have both an adjectival and an adverbial function, but
the adverbial function rarely, if ever, results in any kind of error. The
following sentence, which prompted me to add this explanation, was submitted
by a user of the KISS site:
She dropped her sword and grappled with his knife hand,
trying to free her left arm from the shield so she could
draw her own.
Doesn't, I was asked, "trying" function as an adverb to
"grappled"? The answer to that question is a definite "Yes," but it also
functions as an adjective to "She." Note what happens if we eliminate the
"She":
Her sword was dropped, and there was grappling with his
knife hand,
trying to free her left arm from the shield so she could
draw her own.
The sentence may still be comprehensible, but it is more
difficult to read because it is not as clear as to who is doing the "trying."
Some gerundives have an
entirely adverbial function. For example, "Considering the circumstances,
the case is dismissed." Certainly it is not the case that is doing
the considering. But these gerundives that have no adjectival function
are relatively rare, and when they do occur, the context makes the performer
of the action clear. (In our example, it is obviously the judge who makes
the statement who did the considering.)
Having considered the adverbial
function of gerundives, we can address the question of how KISS helps students
eliminate dangling or misplaced modifiers. First of all, I have serious
reservations about the typical "correct-the-errors" exercises that present
students with sentences that contain the error and expect the students
to make the corrections. I have seen no evidence that such exercises are
effective, and I fear that they may add to the problem. Presenting some
students will visible stimuli of poorly structured sentences may simply
confuse them. If many students are having problems with misplaced modifiers,
teachers may want to put on the board one or two examples and discuss them,
but otherwise, let the KISS Approach itself naturally take care of the
problem.
The KISS psycholinguistic model
explains that every word in any sentence chunks to another word or construction
until everything is chunked to the words in the main S/V/C pattern. The
brain will chunk words as quickly as it can, and thus it will chunk words
and phrases to the nearest word or phrase that in any way makes sense.
This will not be a new idea to students who have been working within a
KISS framework. And, as they analyze sentences, they will be looking for
the adjectival function of gerundives. Thus they will see, over and over
again, that gerundives "chunk" to the performer of whatever the verbal
means. In the errors discussed above, for example, students will quickly
see that "Being of an impulsive nature" chunks to "mother"; that "Thrown
from the car" chunks to "he"; and that "wondering" chunks to "butterflies."
When a dangling or misplaced
modifier appears (and they will) in the writing of a student who is being
trained in the KISS Approach, the teacher can simply write "Ref" (for "reference")
or "SS" (for "sentence structure") in the margin. And the teacher can expect
the student to be able to correct that error without going to a grammar
textbook. In most approaches to grammar, any "Ref" or "SS" in the margins
of a paper might as well be in Greek because the students have not been
taught how sentences work. Note also, that these marks can be meaningful
for students even before they have formally learned what gerundives are.
Assume, for example, that the
writer of "Thrown from the car, he saw her lying on the ground" had not
yet been taught about verbals. In the KISS Approach, the principle of chunking
is taught at Level One, with prepositional phrases, adjectives, and adverbs.
Thus the student does not need to know what a gerundive is. All the teacher
has to do is to point to "Thrown" and ask, "What does it chunk to?" If
the student appears confused, the teacher can follow up with "Who was thrown?"
At that point, most students are smart enough to see that, according to
this sentence, "he" was. And likewise, most students are smart enough to
deal with the occasional gerundive that has an exclusively adverbial function.
Gerunds as Subjects
Some students have problems using
gerunds as subjects. Thus you may find sentences like the following which
was taken from a college Freshman's paper:
By simply making the request that the ladies wear longer
skirts
is not asking too much.
For students who have been working within the KISS Approach,
this error does not have to be directly addressed. All we need to do is
to ask the student to analyze the sentence:
{By simply making[Gerund,
object of "by"] therequest[DO
of "making"] }
[Adj. to "request" thattheladieswearlongerskirts
(DO)] is
not asking
[Gerund, PN of "is"] too much.
In performing this analysis, students find that there
is no subject for the verb, in this case, "is." Realizing this, some students
immediately know how to fix it, but others need an explanation and some
practice with examples.
In every case that I can remember,
the error occurs because the meaningful subject is imprisoned in a prepositional
phrase. The psycholinguistic
model helps students see that, in this case, for example, the reader
will chunk "making," the meaningful subject, in the prepositional phrase.
By the time they get to KISS Level Four, students have had a fair amount
of practice with prepositional phrases, and they have verified for themselves
the rule that objects of prepositions cannot function as the subjects of
verbs that are outside the prepositional phrase. Thus they need to free
the subject ("making") from the phrase. And this is done by simply eliminating
the preposition:
Simply making the request that the ladies wear longer
skirts
is not asking too much.
Errors in using gerunds as subjects are not very common,
but they can be very distracting because they force the reader to have
to reprocess the sentence to find the subject of the verb. In most cases,
however, readers can reprocess and at least determine exactly what the
writer meant. That is not the case with the more frequent, and more serious
errors with misplaced or dangling modifiers.
KISS Level Five (Additional
Constructions)
Remember that constructions are
labeled "Level Five" because they can be taught after everything else has
been basically mastered. In a sense, they are the least important constructions
for understanding how sentences work. Except for an error by teachers (discussed
below), any errors in punctuation involving these constructions are explored
as the students learn to identify the constructions.
Improving Writing
Level 5.1 - Nouns Used as Adverbs; Level 5.2
- Simple Interjections; and Level 5.3 - Direct Address
These are three relatively simple
constructions that most students naturally use. If you have the time, they
can be taught with the KISS Level Two constructions. Stylistically, the
only important point here is probably the KISS explanation of subordinate
clauses that can function as interjections. (For more on this, see KISS
Level 3.2.3 - Interjection? Or Direct Object?)
Some teachers simply tell students
not to use "I." What they probably really mean is not to use first person
pronouns, but most students have not been taught to recognize "first person."
(Teachers can only do so much.) In many writing contexts, however, first
person is not only acceptable but even preferred. What really causes problems
is the use of "I think . . . .," "I believe . . . . ," and "in my opinion."
Professional writers
do occasionally begin sentences with these constructions, but professional
writers understand that whatever they are writing is already understood
to be their opinion. Thus, when they use these constructions, they are,
in effect, flagging whatever it attached to these constructions as their
opinions that they realize are weaker, more open to attack, than the other
ideas in their writing. It is as if they are saying, "I know what is connected
to this is weaker than my other ideas, but I still think it is relevant."
Many student writers, however,
fill their papers with sentences that begin with these constructions. In
effect, the students are saying one of two things -- either they think
that
all their ideas are weak, or they do not understand that the very fact
that they wrote it automatically implies that it is their thought, belief,
or opinion. There is some research (supported by my own experiences with
student writers) that many students feel more comfortable writing this
way. Therefore, forcing students to avoid these constructions in their
drafts probably hinders the students ability to focus on their ideas. But
students can be taught how to edit their writing to eliminate many (or
all) of these constructions. Usually it is simply a matter of deleting
them, but students can also be taught how to use them effectively.
Effective use usually involves
moving them from the beginning of the sentence. "I believe that women are
smarter than men" can be rewritten as "Women are, I believe, smarter than
men." Here again the KISS psycholinguistic model suggests the reason for
the move. In "I believe that women are smarter than men," readers process
"I believe" as the main subject and verb pattern -- the pattern that receives
the most attention. (See the discussion of MIMC, above.) The psychological
model suggests that in "Women are, I believe, smarter than men," readers
will process "Women are" as the main subject and verb -- the pattern that
receives the most focus. The "I believe" then becomes what many linguists
call a "sentence modifier." KISS, in keeping the list of concepts simple,
considers the "I believe" a subordinate clause that functions as an interjection.
Either explanation reduces the focus that is placed on first person. The
writer can still flag the idea as weaker than others, but can do so in
the same way that you will probably find most professional writers doing.
Appositives, post-positioned
adjectives, delayed subjects, and passive voice can be taught immediately
after KISS Level 3.1 (The Basics of Clauses). Note, by the way, that passive
voice could be taught immediately after (or even in) KISS Level One. But
the time that you spend on passive voice will have to be taken away from
the more important question of clauses. (Have you ever seen a grammar textbook
that even discusses what constructions should be taught in which order?)
In the "complete" (grade-level) workbooks, these four constructions are
included for the first time in fifth grade, after KISS Level 3.2.
Level 5.4 - Appositives
Students who read a lot
will probably have assimilated a command of appositives, but students who
do not may have problems using them. Appositives are an important aspect
of a lean, clean style, simply because most appositives can easily been
seen as reductions of S/V/PN clauses:
Loren Eiseley wrote All the Strange Hours.
He was a fossil hunter.
Loren Eiseley, who was a fossil hunter, wrote All
the Strange Hours. Loren Eiseley, a fossil hunter, wrote All the
Strange Hours.
The third example (with the appositive) also illustrates
the importance of appositives in indicating the credibility of an author
in the framing of source material in a research paper. Some college Freshmen,
even after an exercise on appositives, do not seem to be able to use them.
Instead, they will use the two-sentence version, or I have even seen some
students who give the first sentence, then the quotation or paraphrase,
and follow that with a sentence such as "Eiseley was a fossil hunter."
Within the KISS framework, every
high school student should be able to master basic appositives, but how
soon students should be introduced to them needs to be determined by the
art of the teacher.
Level 5.5 - Post-Positioned Adjectives
Whereas the appositive is a reduction
of the S/V/PN pattern, the post-positioned adjective probably develops
as a reduction of the S/V/PA pattern:
The tree fell during the storm. It was old and rotten.
The tree, which was old and rotten, fell during the storm.
The tree, old and rotten, fell during the storm.
The construction surely adds variety to, and even changes
the pace and focus of information in, sentences, but it is not as important
as are appositives.
Level 5.6 - Delayed Subjects and Sentences
The subtle stylistic implications
of delayed subjects and sentences are probably not important enough for
class assignments. I can, however, see someone collecting examples and
comparing them and their contexts to see when and how writers tend to use
them. I'm writing this soon after reading Edgar Allan Poe's "The Case of
M. Valdemar," a story that includes several delayed subjects in the opening
paragraphs. For example, "It is now rendered necessary that I give the
facts
-- as far as I comprehend them myself." In the non-delayed version, this
sentence would read "That I give the facts -- as far as I
comprehend them myself -- is now rendered necessary." Is Poe using the
delayed subjects to distance the narrator from responsibility for what
he is about to explain?
Perhaps, but to really understand
the stylistic implications of this construction, we would probably have
to collect hundreds of examples from early writing. It seems to have the
purpose of emphasizing the complement in the main clause -- "It is true
that he did it." But the construction has become so common, that (if it
was originally used for emphasis) that emphasis has probably been lost.
The stylistic implications of this construction, in other words, are probably
of interest only to the amateur or the specialist.
Level 5.7 - Passive Voice and Retained Complements
Passive voice is not an error,
but there are teachers who tell students not to use it, which is, in effect,
saying that it is erroneous. The trouble with all such instruction is that
it is usually meaningless. "Passive voice" makes sense only to someone
who can identify subjects and verbs. Within KISS, therefore, the primary
focus is on enabling students to identify passive verbs in the first place.
Teachers who want to have students explore the effects of using passives
can easily have students find real texts that include passive voice and
then discuss why the writer may have used it. Ultimately, students can
decide for themselves when it is, and when it is not, an appropriate writing
tool.
Level 5.8 - Noun Absolutes
When they are used as adverbs,
noun absolutes are elegant reductions of subordinate clauses, clauses whose
subjects are not included elsewhere in the sentence:
Bob left. The party became lively.
After Bob left, the party became lively.
Bob having left, the party became lively.
Perhaps this is the place to suggest that students should
probably not be pushed into using appositives, post-positioned adjectives,
or noun absolutes until the students are sufficiently syntactically mature.
All three of these constructions can be viewed as reductions of subordinate
clauses, and there are both theoretical reasons and research that suggest
that students can cognitively master these constructions only after they
have mastered subordinate clauses. Simply put, students' brains have to
master the subordinate clause before they can master how to reduce these
clauses to appositives, post-positioned adjectives, or noun absolutes.
This is a complicated question
-- these constructions can be found in the writing of some very young writers.
Usually, however, these writers are also avid readers. Or, in some cases,
the students have "mastered" a specific type of appositive or noun absolute
as what Roy O'Donnell explained as a "formula" -- a set phrase that the
student has (probably repeatedly) heard or read -- "The game over, we went
home."
Avoiding Errors
By the time they get to KISS
Level Five, students will find that there are few errors left to be dealt
with. Some students do write fragments that consist of appositives, but
they will have learned how to fix these fragments, in most cases, simply
because they will not find and S/V/C pattern in the fragment. If they have
been introduced to the psycholinguistic model, they will understand how
to attach the appositive to the preceding or following sentence.
An Error of Some Teachers
The one serious error that KISS
Level Five addresses is an error made by some teachers. Almost a quarter
of a century ago, when I first taught a grammar course for teachers, we
came across the sentence
The plane crashed five miles from here, its tail pointed
at the sky.
It is a perfectly correct sentence with a noun absolute
after the comma. Several of the experienced teachers in the class, however,
noted that in their students' papers, they were marking sentences like
this as comma-splices. There is nothing worse than teachers telling students
that there are grammatical errors where there are none. |