Back to Main Aluminum Menu | KISS Grammar | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
by Dr. Ed Vavra, Pennsylvania College of Technology Idea Units For almost two decades I have been telling
students that one of the reasons for studying grammar is to improve one's
control over the meaning of what one writes. But what, exactly, is "meaning,"
and more importantly, from the point of view of research, how can one measure
it? I had fumbled with these questions for many years before Rochelle McAndrews,
now a graduate of our Technical Communications program, and also one of
the brightest students ever to grace my classroom, introduced me to the
concept of "idea units" in articles by Carol S. Isakson and Jan H. Spyridakis.
Although the definition of "idea units" used by Isakson and Spyridakis
is too broad for my purposes, the fundamental idea provides a method for
measuring meaning. But before the measurement could in any way be meaningful
in assessing students' writing, I needed a corpus of ideas that were to
be meant. The Aluminum passage provides that corpus.
The Definition of Idea Units In the study reported in "The Influence of Semantics and Syntax on What Readers Remember," Isakson and Spyridakis used two passages from Scientific American of approximately 1,330 words each. They explain how they determined idea units: So that we could measure recalled ideas against ideas in the texts, we quantified the information in each text by separating the texts into idea units (IUs), using a method initially developed by Johnson that we have refined (Spyridakis and Isakson 1998; Wenger and Spyridakis 1993; Spyridakis 1989). IUs are considered to be the smallest information units that readers may logically pause at to emphasize, to enhance meaning, or to take a breath; they are not based on text structure (Johnson 1972). To separate the texts into IUs, 26 students (13 per text) in an advanced technical communication class parsed (separated) all ideas that could be construed as IUs. The students were given the definition of IUs and a sample text with its parsed IUs to help them understand IU parsing. IUs were deemed to exist when a majority of students agreed on the parsing location. As an example of IUs in our study, note the following sentence, excerpted from the Sensors text, and its IU distribution.Although this definition worked for Isakson and Spyridakis, it does have some problems. Apparently, if a subject remembered "insects," but not "arachnids" or "crustaceans," the IU was counted as recalled. But was it? The Aluminum passage, by its very nature, almost eliminates this problem because it is composed of 32 kernel sentences, each of which could be considered as an idea unit. In an attempt, however, to make the analysis even more precise, I divided the eighth sentence into two units "It contains several (8) other substances (9). In addition to simply counting recalled IUs, Isakson and Spyridakis attempted to distinguish more important ideas: Next we determined the importance of the IUs by using a majority tally from the 26 students who, two weeks later, rated the importance of each IU in relation to the overall meaning of the passage. Reading the passage they had not parsed, they labeled more important IUs with the number 1 and less important IUs with the number 2.The relative importance of ideas in a text should be a major concern of anyone who studies grammar, so I found this aspect of their study very interesting. In an earlier study, Spyridakis attempted to study the effects of headings, previews, and/or logical connectives on readers' retention Unfortunately, his definition of "logical connectives" is rather broad: Twenty-two logical connectives (conjunctions, conjunctive adverbs, and conjunctive phrases), chosen from Halliday and Hasan's list of coherence devices [6], were added to each text. Words such as "however," "generally," "in fact," "also," "next," etc.,. were placed among idea units at all levels. {Spyridakis, 398)Once again my curiosity was aroused, but once again I found the definition too broad. Not all logical connectives are equal. I wanted something more specific, and, always the fool, once again I am rushing in. Logical and Rhetorical Relationships
between Idea Units
"Missed" Idea Units The following table indicates how many of the 93 writers omitted each
of the indicated idea units.
Omissions at the End of the Passage The most frequently omitted units (nineteen
times) are numbers 29 ("The metal is light.") and 30 ("It has a luster.").
In part, these omissions result from students either not finishing or rushing
to finish. (In addition to specific revisions that were not completed,
note the high number of omissions beginning at unit 28 and continuing to
the end.) There is, however, at least one other probable reason for some
of these omissions -- beginning with unit 29, the passage may be confusing
and/or redundant.
# 3 "This metal is light, luster, and comes in many forms. The luster of this metal is both bright and silvery."The fact that these five students clearly had problems processing the word "luster" suggests that "luster" probably caused processing problems for other students. These other students may have omitted the term, or, if they handled it correctly, the effort required to do so may have resulted in the omission of "light," "bright," and/or "silvery." That the final unit ("This metal comes in many forms.) was omitted by twelve students was in part, of course, the result of rushing or not finishing, but it may also be the result of confusion about what it means. Does it mean "sheets, ingots, rolls, tubes, and rods"? Given the lack of specific examples, some students may have been confused and left it out as meaningless. The only way to be certain about this, of course, is to question students about what they did and why after they write their revisions. General/Specific Omissions Idea unit # 10 ("Workmen extract these other
substances from the bauxite.") was omitted by eleven students, but its
omission removes no "ideas" from the passage. In effect, unit 10 is a generalized,
introductory statement to the specific ideas presented in units 11 through
19. It is probable that some students felt that this generalization was
unnecessary. If this was, in fact, the case, it raises an interesting question
about readability.
Isakson, Carol S., and Jan H. Spyridakis. "The influence of semantics and syntax on what readers remember." Technical Communication. Aug. 99 Vol. 46, No. 3, 366-381. Downloaded from ProQuest Direct 2/2/00. Spyridakis, Jan. H. "Signaling Effects: Increased Content Retention
and New Answers -- Part II. Journal of Technical Writing and Communication,
Vol. 19 (4), 395-415, 1989.
|